User:Austronesier/sandbox5
Continental West Germanic dialect continuum
[edit]The continental West Germanic dialect continuum encompasses all West Germanic varieties that are spoken on mainland Europe, with the exception of the Frisian languages.[1][2][3]
These West Germanic dialects are traditionally discussed as dialects of Dutch and German depending on the literary language of the area where they are spoken, but they actually form an unbroken dialect continuum: while neighboring varieties are usually mutually intelligible, mutual intelligibility decreases with growing distance.[4]
In spite of its nature as a dialect continuum, the distribution of characteristic innovations allows for a division into distinct dialect groups. On the highest level, three groups can be distinguished: Low Franconian, Low Saxon/Low German and High German. These three groups cut across the boundaries set by modern literary languages: while the great majority of High German dialects are spoken in the region where Standard German serves as literary language, both Low Saxon and Low Franconian are distributed over the German and Dutch literary domains.
Classification
[edit]The continental West Germanic dialects can be broadly divided into three groups, based on linguistic changes that already occurred in the early Middle Ages:
- Low Franconian
- Low Saxon
- High German
High German is set off from the other two groups by the High German consonant shift that affected the tenues *t, *p and *k, with *t shifting to /ts/ or /s/ in all dialects, while the shift of *p and *k is positionally constrained in the northern part of High German area and increasingly affects all positions when moving to the south. The scope of the shift of geminate *pp to /pf/ is used in German dialectology to divide the Upper German subgroup from the Central German subgroup.[5][6][a] Further, High German displays a split of the rising diphthongs *au and *ai underwent a split in High German: they were retained as diphthongs in most environments, but became mid long monophthongs before certain consonants.[8]
Low Saxon (usually called Niederdeutsch 'Low German' or Plattdeutsch in Germany) is primarily characterized by the occurrence of the "unitary plural" (German: Einheitsplural, Dutch: eenheidspluralis): while Proto-Germanic had distinct verb endings for all three persons in the plural, Low Saxon dialects have a uniform ending for all three persons.[9][10][11]
Next to the absence of the High German consonant shift and the (historical) absence of the unitary plural, Low Franconian is defined by the cross-section of two innovations, one of which is shared with Low Saxon, while the other also occurs in High German. Low Franconian and Low Saxon share the (near-)unconditioned monophthongization of *ai and *au to /eː/ and /oː/), although a transitional area in the southeast (=South Low Franconian) takes part the High German split. A feature shared by High German and Low Franconian is the shift of the Proto-West Germanic mid long vowels *ē2 and *ō to falling diphthongs /ie/ and /uo/ (often simplified to /iː/ and /uː/, as in standard Dutch and German). Low Saxon varieties mostly retain the mid monophthongs (although secondary diphthongization occurs in some areas).[8]
These dialect groups are not to be understood in a rigid manner, since each in turn displays a high degree of internal diversification, while dialects spoken in the vicinty, but on opposite sides of the main dividing lines between the major groups are often mutually intelligible. For instance, one can follow a continuous north-to-south sequence of mutually intelligible dialects from Zeelandic (Low Franconian), Brabantian (Low Franconian), Limburgish (Low Franconian), Ripuarian (Central German), Moselle Franconian (Central German), South Franconian (Upper German) to Swabian (Upper German); speakers of each dialect can understand the dialect of their respective immediate neighbors, but will experience difficulties to comprehend more distant dialects. As a result, the choice of larger dialect divisions with sharp borders often becomes arbitrary; only the differences between varieties spoken at the most extremes (e.g. Zeelandic and Tyrolian) intuitively suggest that these varieties must belong to different diasystems (i.e. dialect groups or languages).[4]
Relation to Anglo-Frisian
[edit]The Anglo-Frisian languages form a separate branch within the West Germanic languages. The terms "Ingvaeonic" or "North Sea Germanic" are used to emphasize the connections between innovations found in Anglo-Frisian and the continental Germanic Low German varieties.[12] However, Low German shares a number of features with Low Franconian that are not shared by Anglo-Frisian.[13] Additionally, both Low Franconian and Low Saxon have a mixture of Ingvaeonic features ("Ingvaeonisms") and non-Ingvaeonic features; while a majority of scholars count Low German as part of North Sea Germanic, others dispute its membership. At least at least some of this mixture comes from early and pervasive influence from High German dialects, probably beginning around 700 CE.[14][15] Within Low Franconian, Ingvaeonisms can be further divided into older Ingvaeonisms, which are found through Low Franconian, and younger Ingvaeonisms, which are only found in the coastal areas.[16] Isolated Ingvaeonisms are also found in some West Central German dialects.[17]
"Ingvaeonisms"
[edit]Loss of nasal consonants before spirants
[edit]In North Sea Germanic, a nasal followed by a fricative is lost after a short vowel, resulting in compensatory lengthening of that vowel.[18] This feature is found consistently in the Old Saxon stage of Low German and sometimes in Low Franconian.[19]
Middle Low German dialects restore many nasal consonants lost through the spirant law, giving forms such as ander rather than Old Saxon othar ("other"). In some words, the presence or absence of the nasal fluctuates by dialect, with western dialects using us ("us") while eastern dialects use uns. Some of these changes may be due to leveling of forms with and without the nasal, while others point to High German influence. High German influence on Low Saxon vocabulary is already visible in the Old Saxon period, as Old Saxon attests words such as kind and urkundeo that do not follow the nasal spirant law.[20][21]
Low Franconian shows show cases of the nasal spirant law through its whole dialect area, such as vijf ("five" cf. High German fünf), whereas others are restricted to coastal dialects, such as mui(den), used for river mouths in place names and cognate with standard Dutch mond "mouth".[16]
Central German Ripuarian, Moselle Franconian, and Lorraine Franconian all feature the pronominal form ūs via Ingvaeonic influence. Other n-less forms are also found in these dialects, such as islands where the word Gans lacks an n (e.g. horregeise "wild geese"). Upper Hessian likewise shows gās. However, most cases have been replaced by forms featuring n.[22]
A large area of the southeastern Alemannic dialects has undergone a change similar to the Ingvaeonic nasal spiral law independently, producing forms such as ūs.[23] This change is first attested in the 12th century and had become common by the 13th century.[24]
r-metathesis
[edit]Metathesis of vowel sequences and r has traditionally been considered to be a North Sea Germanic trait, being found commonly in Frisian, slightly less commonly in English. It is a common feature of Northern continental West Germanic dialects, and is found in standard Dutch, but almost entirely absent in standard High German.[25][26] Both Middle Dutch and Middle Low German are securely attested as metathesizing sequences of -rV- (where V = any short vowel) to -Vr- before t, d, s, n in a closed syllable. This creates a contrast with High German words: Dutch borst vs. High German Brust, and in place names, between -born and -bronn/-brunn.[27][28] However, it now appears that metathesis in both languages represents an independent development, with Low German perhaps connected to Frisian, but Dutch developing metathesis first in an unconnected geographic area.[29] Arjen Versloot and Elżbieta Adamczyk argue that metathesis is a common enough linguistic process that it is not a useful diagnostic for Old Saxon's membership in North Sea Germanic.[30]
Metathesis of r clusters is attested in Old Saxon from the 9th century onward, when a Westphalian manuscript attests hers "horse" (cf. High German Ross); however, it is possible that this form is a loanword from Frisian.[28] Other infrequent cases of metathesis have been argued to exist from then onward, especially in forms of names ending in -berht;[31] metathesized forms of the word for "horse" are found in three of the four attested Old Saxon dialects (the fourth does not attest the word), with Westphalian showing a mix of metathesized and non-metathesized forms.[32] From Westphalia, metathesis also spread into the High German Ripuarian and Middle Franconian dialects, and eventually reaching Upper German Lower Alemannic dialects on the Upper Rhine. Metathesis initially follows the same rules as in Westphalian, but it became more infrequent and phonetically restricted as the change moved southward.[33] The metathesized form that has spread the farthest to the south is bersten ("burst"), which has replaced earlier bresten in standard German.[28] In modern High German dialects, metathesized forms are restricted to Central German; earlier metathesized forms have been reversed in favor of unmetathesized ones in many places.[34][35]
For Dutch, metathesis of the same type as Low German is first attested between 1050 and 1150 in Flanders. The number of words affected decreases as one travels East from Flanders; Flemish also has metathesis in some open syllables, such as verde vs. standard Dutch vrede ("peace"). Additionally, the sequence -vR- metathesized if followed by -xt, giving forms such as vrucht rather than High German Furcht ("fright"). This change is found in most Franconian dialects besides Limburgian by the early Middle Dutch period.[27][36]
Palatalization of velars
[edit]The Ingvaeonic languages have a tendency to palatalize the velar consonants /k/ and /ɡ/ before the front vowels /i/ and /e/.[15][37] Scholars disagree whether this feature occurred in a common proto-language stage or developed later, with Old Saxon evidence perhaps suggesting the former.[38]
In Old and Middle Saxon, palatalized forms of /k/ and /ɡ/ (=[ɣ] in most positions) are common, with palatalized /k/ indicated in the orthography by <ki> (e.g. kiennen, cf. High German kennen) or in some cases by <z> (e.g. zind, cf. High German Kind), while palatalized /g/ was indicated by <i(j)> or sometimes <gi> (e.g. ielden, cf. High German gelten).[39] The palatalization of /k/ probably occurred over a wide area and to differing amounts in different dialects; in modern Low German, it has in most but not all cases been reversed to /k/.[40] Outside of many place names, one modern survival is the word sever ("beetle"), still used in many Low German dialects and equivalent to High German Käfer.[41] Earlier /ɡ/, on the other hand, often alternates with /j/ or is a palatal fricative in modern Low German German dialects, often including in the environment of back vowels.[42]
Dutch and German may also show some evidence for far less widespread palatalization of /ɡ/, with older texts showing variation between <g> and <i/j>. However, palatalization is dismissed as a cause by most authorities.[43][44][45]
Fronting of *a
[edit]The fronting of *a is found consistently in Old English and Old Frisian (e.g. Old English dæg vs. Old High German tac "day"), but took place only partially in Low Saxon and Low Franconian, producing doublets of words with a/e in Old Saxon. In Middle Low German, most of these doublets were eliminated in favor of the a version: of the Old Saxon variants glas and gles only glas is found in Middle Low German.[19][46]
Some Dutch dialects also show signs of palatalization of a to e, most often before sp and sk (e.g. modern standard Dutch fles, cf. High German Flasche). This has sometimes been claimed to be an Ingvaeonism, although other factors, such as i-umlaut or analogy, can be used to explain most instances.[47]
Rounding of *a before nasals
[edit]In Anglo-Frisian, a was often rounded to o in front of a nasal consonant, e.g. Old English mon "man". Although this change is attested in Old Saxon with forms such as hond "hand", however, in Middle Low German forms with a have come to dominate. However, in cases where the nasal consonant has been lost before a spirant and the o lengthened, the o vowel remained: gōs "goose" (e.g. modern Eastphalian gous).[48][49][b] Forms with o are also found in Western Dutch dialects of Hollandic, Flemish, and Zealandic in some cases, e.g. sochte "soft" in medieval Flemish (standard Dutch sacht). These forms appear connected to the related change in Anglo-Frisian and Old Saxon.[51]
Sound changes
[edit]Next to the High Germanic consonant shift and the charateristic changes involving Proto-Germamic diphthongs and long mid vowels, several sound changes have affected to various degrees the varieties of the continental West Germanic dialect continuum across the borders between Low Franconian, Low Saxon and High German.
Consonants
[edit]High German consonant shift
[edit]The major divide between High German dialects and the remaining part of the continental West Germanic dialect continuum is defined by the High German consonant shift. In all High German dialects, *t shifted to /ts/ or /s/, while *p and *k became /f/ and /x/ when following a vowel. The northern border of the area where this sound shift occurs is known as the "Benrath line". Reflexes of *p and *k in other positions vary, with the most thorough application of the shift to /pf ~ f/ and /kx ~ x/ occurring in the southern part of the High German dialect area. The scope of the shift of geminate *pp to /pf/ is used in German dialectology to divide the Upper German subgroup from the Central German subgroup; the border between Upper and Central German is called "Speyer line".[5][6]
A related change was the shift of voiced stops to voiceless stops. This is most widespread with *d to /t/, found in all Upper and Central German dialects, with only Rhine Franconian restricting it to geminates and word finally.[52] A related shift, of *b to /p/ is today restricted to southern Bavarian and some Alemannic dialects in initial position, but was formerly found throughout Bavarian and somewhat in Alemannic in all positions in the word.[53] The shift of *g to *k was reversed by the Middle High German period.[54]
Development of Proto-Germanic /β ð ɣ/
[edit]In early West Germanic, the Proto-Germanic voiced obstruents /β ð ɣ/ had the following realizations:[55][56]
- /ð/ shifted to a voiced stop /d/ in all positions.
- /β/ became /b/ in intitial position, in the cluster /-mb-/ and when geminated /-bb-/. Elsewhere, it remained /β/.
- The realization of /ɣ/ is less clear, since the grapheme ⟨g⟩ was used for voiced stops and fricatives alike. A stop realization can safely assumed only following a nasal and under gemination (/-ng-/, /-gg-/), while it most likely remained a fricative in all other positions.[c]
The different dialects show further developments, which generally divide Low German and Low Franconian from High German. In Old Saxon, /β/ appears to have become a stop in the same pattern as described above, however, /ɣ/ appears to have remained a voiced fricative except when following a nasal, based on the evidence of modern dialects.[58] Low Franconian also shifts /β/ as described above, but the status of /ɣ/ in Old Low Franconian is disputed: it was either a voiced fricative in most positions as in Old Saxon, or it had become a stop in initial position and when geminated, only later to spirantized to /ɣ/ again.[59][57] Both Low Franconian and Low German also display final devoicing of the voiced fricatives at the end of words: Old Saxon gaf (from Proto-West Germanic *ɣaβ) and burch (cf. High German Burg).[60][61]
In Old High German, all dialects except Middle Franconian instead are argued to have shifted /β ɣ/ shifted to the stops /b g/ in all positions; Middle Franconian instead shows the same pattern as Old Saxon.[62][d]
Final fortition
[edit]Final fortition or final devoicing refers to the pronounciation of voiced obstruents such as [b, d, g, z, v] as [p, t, k, s, f]. This is a general feature of continental West Germanic languages, and is more general than that found in other Germanic languages, including Old English, where only fricatives are affected.[e] Final devoicing is already attested in Old Low Franconian.[64] There is some evidence for it in Old High German as early as the 8th century, but its presence is only certain in Middle High German, reflected in spellings such as tac for earlier tag. It is likewise attested as the general rule in Middle Low German.[65][66]
Final fortition was lost as a rule in the Early Modern German period due to the syncope of final schwa reintroducing final voiced consonants. However, the rule that final obstruents are devoiced but was reacquired subsequently.[65] A similar development occurred in Low and Central Franconian dialects that apocopated final schwa; most dialects subsequently reacquired the rule of final devoicing, though some also retain voiced final stops.[67] While final fortition is a part of standard German in Germany, it is not part of the Austrian standard, and Low and High German dialects differ in how they handle final contrast. Some dialects have historically retained a contrast, such as Meissen Saxon, others, such as Hessian, instead voice final voiceless stops (see also inner-German lenition).[68]
Voicing of initial and medieval voiceless fricatives
[edit]The Proto-Germanic fricatives /s f þ x/ have historically voiced word internally in all continental Germanic languages, as well as initially in both Low Franconian and High German dialects; /þ/ eventually became a stop d in all positions (see below) and initial and medial /x/ became /h/.[69][70]
In Low Franconian, initial and medial voicing appears to have occurred by the 9th, 10th, or 11th century: a change of /f/ to /v/ is clearly attested by spelling, and a parallel change of /s/ to /z/ can be posited.[71] Middle Dutch continues to show voicing of initial /f/ to /v/, and reflects the voiceless of /s/ to /z/ in spelling (while retaining /s/ in initial consonant clusters such as slapen). It has also been adopted into modern standard Dutch, but in Northern and Western Low Franconian dialects, the initial fricatives are voiceless.[72] While initial voicing is not generally posited for Old Saxon or Middle Low German, both, as well as modern Low German dialects, have medial voicing.[73]
Old High German shows a shift of initial and medieval /f/ to /v/ in the 9th century (fater > vater); evidence of a parallel shift of /s/ to /z/ can be posited from early loanwords in Slavic languages.[70][74] In the late Middle High German period, /v/ merges with /f/ in many dialects, thereby becoming devoiced, although the spelling with <v> continues to be used in standard High German in initial position (Vater, pronounced [faːtɐ]).[54][75] /s/ continues to be voiced as [z] in initial and medial position, but has undergone a number of other changes over the Middle High German period.[76] Conservative Upper German dialects today continue to have distinct lenis consonants /v̥ z̥/ where formerly voiced /z/ and /v/ were present.[70]
Proto-Germanic *þ (/θ/) > /d/
[edit]In all varieties of the continental West Germanic dialect continuum, the Proto-Germamic voiceless dental fricative *þ (/θ/) shifted to /d/ (not counting subsequent sound changes such as final devoicing, rhotacism or complete loss in intervocalic position) during the Old High German, Middle Dutch and Middle Low German periods. After going through an intermediate stage as a voiced dental fricative /ð/, the shift to plosive /d/ started as early as the 6th century in Bavarian. From there, it radiated to neighboring Alemannic (8th century) and East Franconian (9th century), subsequently to Central German (10th–11th century), Low Franconian (12th century) and Low Saxon (12th–14 century).[77][78]
The other West Germanic varieties on the continent, viz. the Frisian languages, were not affected by this change, but eventually lost the dental fricative by different pathways. In West Frisian, it generally became /t/ in initial position and /d/ in other positions by the 15th century. For North Frisian and the Wangerooge dialect of East Frisian, a fricative articluation is still reported in the 19th century, but eventually gave way to a plosive realization in the 20th century in all dialects (except for Amrum North Frisian, which has *þ > /s, z/ in many instances).[79] While not directly related to the sound change in the continental West Germanic dialect continuum, loss of /θ/ in Frisian languages was most probably triggered by contact with West Germanic prestige varieties (Dutch, Low Saxon and standard German) and long periods of bilingualism.[80]
Spirantization and loss of medial voiced stops
[edit]Later spirantization of /b d g/ occurred word internally: /b/ > /w/ occurred in all dialects that experienced inner-German lenition (see below) except for Swabian and some Upper Rhenish varieties, but also throughout Bavarian; /g/ > /ɣ/ occurs in the same area except that only Northern Bavarian and some Central Bavarian is effected; /d/ > /ð/ only occurs in Rhine Franconian.[81] Younger speakers have replaced /ð/ with /r/, e.g. bruːðɐr > bruːrɐr.[82] In Middle High German, these stops might be lost entirely medially, giving variant forms like seit/saget, meide/mägede, and hân/haben. Many of these contracted forms continue to be used in modern dialects.[83]
Middle Dutch likewise frequently contracted medial eg (/eɣ/) or ig (/iɣ/) before n or l.[84]
In modern Dutch, /d/ has been lost after a long vowel or diphthong and a following schwa. In Brabantine, the syllables were kept apart by inserting a semi-vowel /j/ (snīden > snijen), whereas in Hollandish, the following syllable was contracted (weder > weer). Standard Dutch has adopted different forms from different dialects and often continues to write the lost /d/.[85] This loss must have been proceeded by a spirantization of /d/, possibly as /ð/. Many Dutch dialects also lose /ɣ/ and /v/ under the same conditions.[86] Loss of /d/ also affected most Low Franconian dialects in Germany (with the exception of the Bergish dialects) and some western Ripuarian dialects, and further many Low Saxon dialects (Westphalian, Eastphalian, parts of Northern Low German).[87]
Developments of sibilants and sibilant clusters
[edit]West Germanic originally contained a consonant cluster *sk in all positions of the word; continental West Germanic varieties have shifted this cluster in all or some of these positions. In High German dialects, *sk shifted to /ʃ/ in High German dialects, probably via an intermediate stage of /sx/; this is usually dated to the 11th century.[88] The situation is more complicated in other West Germanic dialects, which treated *sk differently depending on its position in the word.[89] In most Low Franconian dialects, *sk shifted to /sx/ in initial position but simplified to /s/ in medial and final position; most scholars believe this change occurred in the Middle Dutch period (1150-1500).[90] Western Flemish dialects did not simplify medial /sk/ to /s/: the cluster appears there variously as /sk/, /sʔ/, /ʃx/, and /ʃ/,[91] while some northern Low Franconian dialects maintained initial /sk/.[92] Low German dialects vary in how they shifted /*sk/. East Low German and Eastphalian show /ʃ/ in all positions like High German, whereas Westphalian and North Low German show a variety of outcomes, shown on the table below.[93] The Low German changes probably postdate the shift in High German.[94]
Dialect group | Initial *sk | Medial *sk | Final *sk |
---|---|---|---|
High German | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ |
Southeastern Low Franconian | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ |
East Low German | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ |
Eastphalian Low German | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ | /ʃ/ |
North Low German and Westphalian | /ʃ/ | /sk/ | /sk/ |
/ʃ/ | /s/ | /s/ | |
/ʃx/ | /sk/ | /sk/ | |
/ʃx/ | /ʃk/ | /ʃx/ | |
/ʃx/ | /ʃk/ | /s/ | |
/sx/, /sç/ | /sk/ | /sk/ | |
Western Flemish[f] | /sx/ | /sk/, /sʔ/, /ʃx/, /ʃ/ | /s/ |
Most Low Franconian | /sx/ | /s/ | /s/ |
With the emergence of this new /ʃ/-sound, the old sibilant phoneme /s/ (which probably had a retracted [ʃ]-like articulation in Old and Middle High German, as still witnessed by modern Dutch) merged with /ʃ/ in word-initial position before a consonant (e.g. slange > Schlange /ʃlaŋə/ 'snake', stein > Stein /ʃtai̯n/ 'stone') in all High German dialects, and also in some Low Saxon and South Low Franconian dialects. The shift of /rs/ to /rʃ/ is common in most High German dialects, but only sporadically found its way into standard High German (e.g. ars > Arsch 'arse'). In Alemannic and adjacent Upper Franconian, Rhine Franocnian and Moselle Franconian dialects, /sp/ and /st/ became /ʃp/ and /ʃt/ also in non-initial position (e.g. fest > fescht 'tight').[g] In the southmost part of Alemannic and Bavarian Upper German, /s/ could even become /ʃ/ when not appearing before a consonant (e.g. sî > /ʃiː/ 'she' in the Wallis dialect of Visp).[96][97]
Development of West Germanic *xs
[edit]The West Germanic consonant cluster *xs does not remain in any continental West Germanic language. It has developed in two ways: simplification to /s/ or the change of the velar fricative /x/ to a velar stop /k/ (giving /ks/).
Generally speaking, all Low German and Low Franconian dialects have simplified *xs to /s/ (e.g. Low German ses, Dutch zes = six).[98] This simplification occurred as early as the 9th century in Low Franconian, from there spreading eastward before the Middle Low German period.[99][100][101] Various Central and Upper German dialects, including several that do not directly border each other, show the same simplification, with forms such as wassen for wahsen attested in Middle Franconian, Hessian, and sometimes in Alemannic already in the Middle High German period.[102][103]
In those High German dialects that don't simplify *xs to /s/, /*xs/ instead becomes /ks/ (e.g. seks = six).Cite error: There are <ref>
tags on this page without content in them (see the help page). This change appears to have begun in Bavarian in late Middle High German. By the 16th century, the /ks/ pronunciation was also found in East Central German, from there spreading under the influence of the standard. The /ks/ pronunciation has replaced the earlier simplification to /s/ in most dialects, leaving only a few words or place names in most.[103][102][104]
/ft/ > /xt/
[edit]Various dialects of Low Franconian, Low German, and West Central German dialects share a shift of /f/ to /x/ when preceding /t/ and following a short vowel. Generally, these dialects are found on the Western edge of the West Germanic continuum.[105]
The earliest evidence for the shift comes from Middle Franconian from the 9th century, this suggests a spread from Central Germany; from there the change spread north and east, growing weaker as it goes.[106] It is attested in Low Saxon in the 10th and Low Franconian in the 12th centuries.[107] Among the Low Franconian dialects, North Hollandic was excluded from the change, and relicts of /ft/ can be found in Northern Brabantine as late as the fifteenth century.[99] Within Low German, the shift is most prevalent in Westphalian. While some shifted words are found throughout the Low German area, such as lucht (High German Luft "air"), most dialects of Low German retain /ft/ in most words.[108]
Inner-German lenition
[edit]Inner-German lenition refers to the weakening of fortis stops and fricatives to their equivalent lenis stops and fricatives (/p, t, k, s, f/ > /b, d, g, z, v/), often with the loss of aspiration. It is first attested around around 1300 near Vienna in the Bavarian dialect group before spreading to other dialects.[54][109][h] It is referred to as "inner-German" because it is not found in dialects in the north of Central German, nor in southern Upper German dialects.[110] All Central German dialects except for Northern Thuringian, Middle Franconian, and Silesian are affected. Affected Upper German dialects include Swabian, East Franconian, and Central and Northern Bavarian. The Bavarian realization of the lenition differs from that found in the other dialects.[111][112] Between the Upper German dialects with lenition and those without is a thin belt of dialects that only weaken consonants in initial position.[112]
In the non-Bavarian dialects, the change applies to all instances of /p t k/ in all positions, causing them to lose aspiration and merge with /b d g/. In East Franconian, for instance, pass and Bass, Tier and dir, and Karten and Garten are all pronounced with the lenis stops. The shift also affects affricates such as /st, sp, ts/ and /pf/, which become /ʃd, ʃb, ds/ and /bf/. Final /sp/ and /mp/ may not be weakened.[113][82][114]
In the Bavarian dialects in initial position, the same change happens and also includes affricates /pf ts kx/ gnobf (standard Knopf). However, /k/ retains its aspiration and is thus distinct for /g/: Lower Austrian gʰuːɐ (standard German Kuh). Word-internally and finally, Central and Northern Bavarian do not weaken /p, t, k, s, f/ before fricatives and stops, thus retaining combinations such as /st, sp, pf, ks, ft, cht, ts/ and /mp/. Additionally, these dialects shorten vowels before geminates, which remain fortis, while lengthening vowels whenever /p, t, k/ were not geminated while also weakening them. All sounds, including /st, pf, cht, ts/, are voiced finally except for /ks/ and /mp/.[115][116] The result is that lenis consonants always appear after short vowels and fortis consonants always appear after long vowels. Due to apocope, morphological variations can be shown via a change in consonant and vowel length: tiːʒ̊ (table) vs. tiʃː (tables).[117]
Loss of final nasal consonants
[edit]Final nasal consonants are often deleted in West Germanic dialects, most commonly in unstressed syllables but also occasionally in stressed ones.
Final -n is preserved in all monosyllabic words featuring a long vowel or diphthong in Low German, Rhine Franconian, and Upper Saxon, and most of Thuringian. Northern and Central Franconian dialects show variability in whether they delete final unstressed /n/ after schwa, giving variously [-ə] and [-ən]. Moselle Franconian follows the so-called "Eifel rule" (Eifler Regel) in the inflection of masculine articles and adjectives.[118] Further south except in Bavarian, however, n is deleted in progressively more instances. In more southerly Central German dialects, forms such as mein "my" may become mei (Hessian). All Alemannic dialects also delete final unstressed /n/. In far southern Alemannic, all final /n/'s are deleted, including on monosyllabic words. This gives words such as Maa (Mann) "man" and schö (schön) "beautiful".[119][120]
In standard Dutch and many Dutch dialects, including Hollandic, Brabantine, and Limburgs, a similar process occurs, but only /n/ after a schwa is deleted; western Flemish and Low Fraconian dialects spoken close to the Frisian and Low Saxon speech areas retain the final -n of the suffix and instead tend to delete the schwa and create a syllabic nasal. Zeelandish deletes some nasals and retains others.[121][119][122] The Low Franconian Limburgs and Central German Ripuarian dialects share a feature of deleting final /n/ after lax vowels in monosyllabic words.[123]
Velarization
[edit]West Germanic dialects feature two main types of consonant velarization: the change of /nd/ to /ŋd/ and /nt/ to /ŋ/, /ŋg/, or /ŋk/, and "Rhenish gutturalization" (or "velarization"), which affects final nasals and dental stops after a historically long vowel.[124]
The more common of the two velarizations is the change of /nd/ and /nt/, where it mostly occurs word internally, for instance Ende > Enge or Engde. The affected word with the greatest geographic spread is hinten (behind), which appears as hinge(n) East to West in various Central German dialects from the Rhineland to the German-Polish border, and also affects Alemannic dialects as far as Switzerland. It was also formerly present in High Prussian and affects some Alemannic dialects further south in the Rhineland, but mostly in pockets. The change at the end of a word (e.g. Kind > Kingd) has a more restricted geographic distribution, found in Ripuarian, and to some extent in Moselle Franconian, Eastern Hessian, and a pocket in Silesian.[125][126] Southern Middle Low German dialects also experienced this process, including southern Westphalian, southern Brandenburgish, parts of Pommeranian and Low Prussian.[127]
"Rhenish gutturalization" is found in Ripuarian and some surrounding areas, including some parts of Limburgish. In "Rhenish gutturalization," dental stops and nasals are velarized after a historic long vowel, typically ī, ǖ, and ō, which is then shortened and possibly lowered: wīn > weŋ, brūn > broŋ, liute > lük, zīt > tsek.[128][129][130]
Pronunciation of /r/
[edit]Germanic languages show a variety of realizations of /r/. Early Germanic is often argued to have possessed an apical trill r (r), a type of r still found in modern Germanic dialects; however Germanic languages appear to have always possessed more than one realization of /r/.[131]
Among tradition dialects of German, most dialects had an apical trill (r), as formerly also found in the standard. Parts of the historical Silesian dialect had a retroflex rhotic (ɽ) or similar sound. The uvular frictative (ʁ), today considered standard in Germany, was historically only found in some regions of East Central German. Dialects in the West, including most Swabian and High Alemannic, some Moselle Franconian, and some Rhine Franconian, had the related uvular trill (ʀ. While scholars have often argued that this form of /r/ only entered German late and under French influence, this view lacks significant support. Uvular /r/ is found in dialects not significantly influenced by French. Additionally, Yiddish has ʁ, pointing to an origin before the separation of High German and Yiddish.[132][133] In the present day, uvular /r/ has become far more widespread. Among contemporary dialects, some varieties devoice, particularly in the West, final /r/ to x in words like Sport.[134]
Degemination
[edit]Degemination refers to the simplification of geminate (long) consonants to single (short) consonants. This change is closely associated with open syllable lengthening in vowels, as syllables featuring geminates were closed and thus produced short vowels in those dialects that underwent the process. In modern continental West Germanic, geminate consonants only remain in southern Alemannic and Bavarian dialects. All other dialects have simplified geminate stops to single stops.[135] Currently, central and southern Alemannic dialects are in the process of losing distinct phonemic gemination.[136] Although standard German and Dutch no longer have geminate consonants, the spelling has often been maintained to distinguish long and short vowels.[137]
The continental West Germanic languages experienced degemination mostly in their middle periods. Low Franconian dialects experienced degemination sometime after the end of the Old Dutch period, either somewhat after or in parallel to open syllable lengthening.[138] Geminates were also lost during the Middle Low German period. Among Central and High German dialects that have lost geminates, the change is dated later than in Dutch, toward the end of the Middle High German period, also in a relative chronology with open syllable lengthening.[54] Most High German dialects underwent an earlier partial degemination in the Old High German period: they simplified geminate consonants created by the High German consonant shift after long vowels (e.g. slāffen > slāfen). Western Central Bavarian and southern Alemannic dialects have retained geminates in these positions as well.[139]
Nasal/liquid + stop clusters
[edit]Assimilation of medial -mb- to -mm- (originally a geminate) is already attested in the Old Low Franconian period; the change likewise began in Old Saxon and was completed before the Middle Low German period.[140] Among High German dialects, it is first attested in Central German dialects early in the Middle High German period, spreading south into Bavarian by around 1300. In some modern Austro-Bavarian and Swiss Alemannic dialects, the assimilation has not occurred, giving forms such as Aargau xambə "comb". The change of final -mb (usually /mp/ with final devoicing) is not attested until later, as a leveling of the difference between e.g. lamp "lamb" and lammes "lamb's".[141][142] Many Eastern High German dialects retain final -mb, including Bavarian, Thuringian, some East Franconian, Upper Saxon, and Silesian (e.g. Thuringian lɔmp "lamb").[143] Limburgish likewise retains finally -mb, e.g. kromp "bent, crooked", as do the neighboring Bergish dialects.[144][145]
The simplification of medial -ng- (phonetically /ŋg/) to ŋ appears to have occurred in or after the Middle High German, Middle Low German, and Middle Dutch periods respectively.[24] In Swiss Alemannic, the change appears as a assimilation of the g to the previous [ŋ], giving geminated forms like tsuŋŋə "tongue". A few southern Austro-Bavarian and Swiss dialects retain the original /ŋg/ pronunciation, e.g. Southern Bavarian tsuŋg (with schwa apocope). In Limburgish, Zealandic, southwestern Dutch dialects, as well as all of Low German, Central Franconian, Low Hessian and much of Upper Hessian, and some East Franconian dialects, final -ng continues to be pronounced /ŋk/; in others, it has simplified to [ŋ].[146][147][i]
In all Low German dialects, earlier medial -nd- has assimilated to simple -n-, but the cluster is retained word finally: Hamburg North Low German kinr̩ "children", but kint "child". This change can be dated either to late Old Saxon or early Middle Low German.[149] In Old High German, the earlier cluster -nd- had shifted to -nt- as a result of the High German consonant shift. In Middle High German, however, the t is partially assimilated to -d-, returning -nd-.[150][151] All Central German dialects as well as South Franconian also have further simplified -nd-, as in Low German, although in northern and eastern Central German it only occurs in cases where velarization has not occurred. In much of Hessian, simplification also occurs word-finally (e.g. khin "child"), whereas in other dialects it only occurs intervocalically. Among Upper German dialects, assimilation of -nd- to -nn- is only found in western Swiss Alemannic.[152]
All Low German and Central German as well as some Upper Franconian dialects also feature a simplification of medial -ld- to -l-, e.g. Hamburg North Low German ʃulɐ "shoulder". At the end of a word, -ld- is retained. In Low German, this change can be traced to early Middle Low German or late Old Saxon.[149] In Upper Franconian, the change is limited to individual words; one word, bal (< older balde), has penetrated as far south as Swiss Alemannic. Low German and some East Central German dialects also simplify medial -rd- to -r-, e.g. Thuringian aːrə "earth". The change is also found in some words in West Central German.[153] Middle High German experienced the same change as with earlier -nt- in these same environments (partial assimilation of t to d).[154]
Vowels
[edit]Umlaut and umlaut blocking
[edit]i-Umlaut refers to a process of vowel raising and/or fronting of vowels when i/j occurred later in the word. Umlaut processes can be divided into three categories: "primary umlaut", that is, the raising of short /a/ to /e/ when followed later in the word by /i/ or /j/ except in certain "blocking" environments; "secondary umlaut", the raising of /aː/ to /æː/ before /i-j/ and of /a/ to /æ/ in "blocking" environments; and then "general umlaut", the fronting of /u(ː)/ and /o(ː)/ to /y(ː)/ and /ø(ː)/.[155][j] In individual dialects, umlaut effects are sometimes found on other vowels or diphthongs.[157] In some Upper German dialects, the resulting vowels from primary and secondary umlaut of West Germanic /a/ remain distinct from the original West Germanic /e/ (often written ⟨ë⟩ by linguists); in Low German Westphalian, the products of primary and secondary umlaut remain distinct vowels. In other dialects, the resulting vowels have merged to a single vowel.[158] The vowels resulting from "general umlaut" have experienced unrounding in most High German, but not most Low German dialects (on the situation in Dutch, see below).
While all West Germanic dialects show "primary umlaut", not all show "secondary" or "general umlaut".[159] Among the modern dialects, the two areas lacking some umlaut, namely Low Franconian and Upper German, are on opposite sides of the West Germanic dialect continuum.[160] Scholars are divided as to whether this lack of umlaut is a secondary development (the removal of umlaut via analogy, for instance), or means that these dialects never experienced umlaut in this instances. Scholars also debate the exact manner in which i-umlaut occurred: whether it was a progressive sound change beginning with primary umlaut and then only later spreading to secondary and general umlaut in historic times, or whether the process had already occurred in common Northwest Germanic and was not marked in writing. In the earliest attested forms of High German, Old Low Franconian, and Old Saxon, only "primary umlaut" is marked in writing, although in Old High German and Old Saxon, other vowels must have experienced umlaut.[161][162]
Most western Low Franconian dialects, as well as Standard Dutch, completely lack the umlaut of long vowels (/aː/, /oː/, /uː/).[163][k][165] Additionally, many Low Franconian varieties block primary umlaut in cases where /x/ and a consonant intervene between the /a/ and /i-j/: thus standard Dutch machtig vs. standard German mächtig.[163] As one travels eastward from the coast, one encounters progressively more umlaut effects, with first /aː/ and then /oː/ also being affected.[163] Thus, coastal Western Flemish varieties lack "secondary umlaut" of /aː/, a feature shared with the dialects of North and South Holland, whereas Flemish Dutch varieties further to the east do have "secondary umlaut" of /aː/.[166] Finally, "General umlaut" of West Germanic /*oː/ and /*au/ is only encountered in dialects farther east, with the exception of the area around Utrecht.[164] The more limited application of umlaut in Dutch results in contrasts between standard Dutch and standard German such as kaas vs. Käse, horen vs. hören, and groeten vs. grüßen.[167] Low Franconian varieties that have general umlaut can be further divided into dialects which only have umlaut as a historical sound shift in the lexicon, and those which have umlaut as a part of inflectional morphology (e.g. plural formation, verb conjugation). This morphological umlaut is restricted to South Low Franconian (e.g. Limburgish /vuːt/ 'foot' vs. /vyːt/ 'feet') and to the easternmost part of the Brabantian and Kleverlandish dialect areas. Dutch Low Saxon varieties display the same division: while all have general umlaut in individual words, morphological umlaut is restricted to the eastern dialects.[168][169]
The southern High German dialects Bavarian and Alemannic show more umlaut than Low Franconian, but also have a number of forms where "general umlaut" is lacking.[164] This is particularly true of Bavarian..[170] In Alemmanic and Bavarian, umlaut of /u/ is regularly blocked when formerly geminated /kk/ or /xx/ intervene between it and /i-j/: southern Upper German muck, ʃduk, khuxɘ, lu:ge vs. standard German Mücke, Stück, Küche, Lüge.[167][163] In the Appenzeller dialect, umlaut of /u/ also fails before geminate /mm/,[171] while Alemannic also sometimes blocks umlaut of /u/ before intervening /pf/, /kx/, and /ts/.[172] In a smaller area of Alemannic and Austro-Bavarian, the umlaut of of the diphthong /ou/ is blocked: drõːmə vs. standard German träumen. Southern East Franconian shows a less consistent application of umlaut blocking, e.g. mugə (Mücke) but kʰyxə (Küche).[173] Modern standard German has adopted some forms featuring umlaut-blocking, including suchen (cf. Low German sööken) and um (from Old High German umbi, cf. Low German öm).[174]
Development of *au and *ai
[edit]The Proto-Germanic diphthongs *ai and *au fully or partially undergo monophthongization to ē and ō in Continental West Germanic, in contrast to Anglo-Frisian, where reflexes as ā and ēa dominate.[175]
In Old Saxon, *au is always reflected as ō.[15][l] In Middle Low Saxon, this o-sound is generally spelled in the same way as the reflex of original *ō. Yet, it must have been phonetically different since it has reflexes distinct from the latter in many modern Low Saxon dialects; for this reason it is notated as ō² in scholarly works (e.g. eastern Westphalian brō²t > /braut/ 'bread' vs. kō¹ken > /kɛokən/ 'cake', with ō¹ < *au).[177]
Most dialects of Old Low Franconian show a parallel development to Old Saxon, with *au becoming /ō/ except when *au was followed by *w, in which case /ou/ was produced (e.g. houwen "to hew"); this change probably dates to the beginning of the 8th century.[178] This ō vowel shows various outcomes in modern Dutch and Low German dialects, including diphthongization in many.[179][180] Old High German, on the other hand, retains a diphthong in most cases, shifting *au to /ou/ except when *au occurred before /x/ or a dental consonant, in which case it also become /ō/.[181] In southern German Low Franconian, Middle Franconian up to Moselle and Rhine, most of Low Hessian and all of Upper Hessian, as well as the eastern half of Thuringian and almost all of Upper Saxon, this earlier ō has since been raised to uː. Dialects on the edges of this area have further diphthongized [uː] to [uːə], [uːɐ], or [uo] (Mülheim an der Ruhr bruːɐt "bread"). In southern Moselle Franconian, ō is palatalized to ē before ç (Luxemburgish hēç "high").[182]
Proto-Germanic *ai split into *ē and *ei in Old Saxon, Old Low Franconian and Old High German, however under different conditions. In Old Saxon and most of Old Low Franconian, the monophthong *ē is the regular outcome unless the following syllable contained /i/ or /j/, in which case the diphthong *ei occurred.[183] Middle and Modern Dutch still reflect the original distribution of these sounds. French and West Flemish, however, almost always has ē (usually [ɪə] in modern dialects), with East Flemish forming a transition area.[184] [ɪə] is also found in North Hollandic; part of West Flemish has [ɛː].[185] In Middle Low Saxon, both were often spelled alike, but are distinguished from each other (and from two other sources of ⟨ē⟩) in academic notation as ē² (corresponding to Low Franconian *ē) and ē³ ((corresponding to Low Franconian *ei). The former is mostly reflected as /eː/ in modern dialects, while the latter usually has become /ai/, e.g. hē²t > /heːt/ 'hot' and rē³n > /rain/ 'pure' in Münsterland Westphalian.[177][186] Dutch Low Saxon likewise shows a diphthong, [ai] in central Groningen and [ei] in the remainder of Groningen and some other areas.[185]
In Old High German, *ai generally appears as ei and only shifted to ē before in r, w, h and in absolute final postion. This type of split extends to the north beyond the Benrath line into the southeastern part of the Low Franconian area and is one of the features that defines the South Low Franconian dialect group.[187][183][m] In the same group of dialects that raised ō to [uː], this earlier ē has since been raised to iː, with some dialects, such as southern German Low Franconian and Ripuarian, as well as some West Thuringian dialects, having diphthongized the resulting vowel to either [iːə] or [iːɐ] (Mülheim an der Ruhr sniːɐ "snow"). Additionally, many Upper German and neighboring Central German dialects have diphthongized ē to [ɛi], [æi], ae, or [ɛə].[189]
Beginning in the 12th century, earlier ei and ou were lowered to [ai] and au in many High German dialects.[190] This change spread from Bavarian northwards. In those dialects with this change that have also experienced a diphthongization of old ī and ū, the resulting diphthongs are also distinguished, although in standard German they are pronounced the same (as [ai] and [au]).[191] Many dialects have maintained diphthongal realizations of ei and ou is retained in many High German dialects, often with further modifications such as ei > /ɔi/ (Swabian) or /ɔa/ (Central Bavarian). Secondary monophthongization is also widespread; its outcome is usually distinct from the original monophthongs. E.g., Central Hessian has ei, au > /aː/, while Ripuarian dialects have /eː/ and /oː/. In both dialect groups (and also in East Franconian and Upper Saxon dialects), the original monophthongs ē and ō were raised to /iː/ and /uː/.[192][193][n] Dialects with secondary monophthongization vary as to whether they monophthongized the earlier, higher Middle High German ei and ou or the later [ai] and [au]. Generally, monophthongization did not take before a vowel, word finally, or in the combination ouw.[194]
Development of Proto-West Germanic *ē² and *ō
[edit]Early West Germanic had two long mid vowels, *ē² and *ō. While *ō was a straightforward continuation of the Proto-Germanic vowel, *ē² had developed from various sources the details of which are disputed. Original Proto-Germanic *ē had shifted to *æ in Northwest Germanic and was further lowered to *ā in Continental West Germanic (except for isolated Ingvaeonisms in Low Saxon and Low Franconian).[195]
In Old Low Franconian and Old High German, *ē² and *ō became falling diphthongs /ie/ and /uo/, with the latter also having an umlauted variant, /üe/, in High German.[195][196] These are generally retained as falling diphthongs in Alemannic and Bavarian dialects (/iə/, /uə/, and /yə/ in Alemannic, /ia/, /ua/, and /ya/ in Bavarian), while they became high monophthongs /iː/, /uː/ /yː/ in East Franconian, most Low Franconian and many Central German dialects (including standard Dutch and standard German). In Moselle Fraconian, Ripuarian Fraconian and South Low Franconian, the most common reflexes are /eː/ and /oː/.[o] In North Bavarian, Central Hessian and some Moselle Franconian dialects, the rising diphthongs[p] /ei/ and /ou/ appear.[197][198][199]
In Low Saxon, the long mid vowels mostly appear unchanged in the early literary record of Old Saxon (although they are occasionally spelled ie and uo).[195] In Middle Low German, they are also generally written as mid monophthongs wihouth being distinguished from mid monophthongs from other sources. In academic descriptions of Middle Low German, the reflexes of *ē² and *ō are conventionally transcribed as ē⁴ and ō¹, based on the historical source and and the various realizations of these mid monophthongs in modern Low Saxon dialects. They appear as /eː/ and /oː/ in northern Low Saxon dialects, but became diphthongs in large parts of Westphalian and Eastphalian, e.g. kō¹ken > /kaukən/ 'cake', flē⁴gen > /flaigən/ 'fly (v.)' in southern Westphalian.[177][186] In Low Saxon varieties spoken on both sides close to the Dutch–German border, the monophthongs are raised to near-high /ɪː/ and /ʊː/, while they have become high vowels /iː/ and /uː/ in most Dutch Low Saxon dialects spoken further west.[q][200][201]
Developed of West Germanic *eu
[edit]The Proto-West Germanic diphthong *eu develops in different ways in the different dialects. Most dialects of High German, Low Franconian, and Low Saxon follow the "Franconian rule," according to which *eu lowered to /io/ before a low vowel in the next syllable except before /w/, and became /iu/ elsewhere: *beuda > OHG biotan. In Bavarian and Alemannic, however *eu was only lowered to /io/ if not before a labial or velar consonant except h, otherwise it became /iu/, regardless of the following vowel: *beuda > biutan.[202][203]
In all dialects, /io/ merged with reflexes of Proto-West Germanic *ē². Most upper German dialects have retained earlier /iu/ as a separate diphthong until the present. Other dialects have instead merged it with /yː/ or /uː/ in the High Middle Ages; however, it was rediphthongized in North Bavarian and Hessian to /öu/.[202][203][204] In southern Low Franconian dialects, the reflexes of earlier /io/ and /iu/ have both merged to /ie/, something also found in more northern Low Franconian dialects before /r/.[205]
Diphthonization of long high vowels
[edit]The long high vowels *ī and *ū (and also umlauted *ǖ /yː/) independently became diphthongs in two large disjunct areas of the continental West Germanic dialect continuum (and also in the West Germanic Anglic varieties on the British Isles): in the southeastern part having radiated out from the Bavarian to much of the High German area; and in large parts the Low Franconian area in the northwest, with the Brabantian dialect as a historical starting point. Both standard Dutch and standard German are based on varieties that have undergone the diphthonization of high vowels, as in Dutch ijs /ɛis/ and German Eis /ais/ from Middle Dutch/Middle High German īs 'ice'.[206]
Among High German dialects, diphthongization is first documented in Bavarian dialects in Carinthia and Tyrol starting from the 12th century. By the 15th century, it had reached its current extent.[207] The diphthongization area covers all of Bavarian, East Franconian, most of Rhine Franconian, Moselle Franconian and East Central German, and the Swabian branch of the Alemannic dialect group. The high vowels remained unchanged in almost all Alemannic varieties spoken to the west and south of Swabian (with small isolated pockets of diphthongizing dialects in the High Alemannic and Highest Alemannic areas), in adjacent Rhine Franconian and Moselle Franconian dialects spoken in Lorraine, in the Moselle Franconian dialects of the Siegerland, in Ripuarian, and in a contiguous area formed by Hessian and Thuringian dialects spoken on both sides of the divide between West and East Central German.[208]
In the Low Franconian area, the diphthongization started in the early Modern Dutch period (i.e. the 1500s) in the Brabantian and East Flemish areas, and from there spread to the north to Holland and Utrecht.[209] The old long high vowels were retained in the southwest in West Flemish and Zeelandic,[209] and in Kleverlandish and most of South Low Franconian in the east (except for much of West Limburgish).[210][211]
In Low Saxon, long high vowels remained for the most part unchanged. Diphthonization only occurred (independent of the Low Franconian and High German vowel shifts) in a limited area comprising eastern Westphalian dialects and neighboring dialects of the Eastphalian group, e.g. hūs > /hius/ 'house', īs > /uis/ 'ice'.[212]
The common West Germanic drift towards diphthonization of high vowels can be explained by assuming that *ī and *ū where phonetically [ij] and [uw] in the entire West Germanic area and thus inherently prone to diphthongization. This is corroborated by the fact that in Ripuarian and in the non-dipthongizing varieties of South Low Franconian, high long vowels behave like rising diphthongs (and also like combinations of short vowels with syllable-closing sonorants /l, r, m, n/) in respect to their tone accent properties.[206]
Reflexes of ā (< Proto-Germanic *ē¹)
[edit]The Proto-Germanic vowel *ē¹ lowered to *ǣ in Proto-(North-)West Germanic and became a long open vowel ā /aː/ in Old Saxon, Old Low Franconian and Old High German.[213] With the exception of western Low Franconian varieties, this ā underwent a split in the late Middle Ages into a non-umlauted form and an umlauted form (usually transcribed as æ in descriptions of Middle High German, and as ē¹ for Middle Low German).[214]
In the non-umlauting Low Franconian area, many coastal dialects display spontaneous fronting to /ɛː/~/æː/ (in Zeeland, parts of West Flanders and South Holland) or /eː/ (North Holland at the language border to Frisian). Further inland in Holland, /aː/ remains unchanged, while in non-umlauting Low Franconian varieties in the south, "darkened" (i.e. velarized) reflexes as /ɔː/ and /oː/ occur.[215][216]
In High German, Low Saxon and the umlauting Low Franconian dialects, ā underwent velarization to /ɑː/, /ɔ:/, /o:/ or even /u:/, often merging with back vowels from other sources such as monophthongized *au, or lengthened *o or *u in open syllables. Only in Cimbrian, some Highest and High Alemannic dialects, and the East German dialect of Berlin, /aː/ retained its original quality.[217] There are two umlauted counterparts of ā. In words like Middle High German kæse 'cheese' or schære 'scissors' (or its Low Saxon and Low Franconian cognates), umlauted ā appears in the base form of the word and does not alternate with non-umlauted ā. In such words, æ usually merged with front vowels from other sources to /ɛː/, /eː/ or even /iː/. In Bavarian (excluding Cimbrian), Alsatian and many East Central German dialects, æ is represented by /aː/. Only few dialects retain umlauted ā as a distinct sound that does not merge with any other sound, e.g. /ɛː/ in southern Westphalian (where it is the front counterpart of /ɔ:/ from non-umlauted ā).[218][219] The other umlauted counterpart of ā (so-called Analogieumlaut, lit. 'umlaut by analogy') appears in inflected forms (e.g. noun plurals, or 2nd and 3rd person singular verb forms) and often differs from primary reflexes of æ. In dialects that have a rounded reflex of ā, its counterpart in inflected forms is also rounded (except in dialects with unrounding), e.g. Kleverlandish Low Franconian /ʃœːp/ 'sheep', plural of /ʃɔːp/.[220][221]
Unstressed vowel reduction
[edit]All the continental West Germanic languages have greatly simplified the vowel and syllable structure of Proto-West Germanic. The tendency of Germanic languages to reduce vowels in unstressed syllables in Germanic languages has commonly been attributed to Germanic's strong word-initial or root syllable stress. This has resulted in most unstressed vowels becoming schwa [ə] over time.[222][223]
The earliest recorded stages of West Germanic languages all contain a variety of full vowels in unstressed syllables. Old High German even retains long vowels in unstressed syllables, which are not found in the other early-attested West Germanic languages.[224] The change from the old to the middle stage of High and Low German and Dutch was marked by the widespread replacement of vowels in unstressed syllables with schwa, resulting in simplifications of the morphology as endings become indistinct. However, some endings with secondary stress maintained full vowels, such as -bar, -dom, and -unge.[225][226] Additionally, unstressed medial syllables could be lost entirely through syncope: OHG hêriro > MHG hêrre (modern German Herr).[227]
The process began in unstressed prefixes before progressing to medial and then to final syllables. Old Low Franconian in the 9th and 10th centuries appears to have experienced far greater reduction than contemporary Old High German or Old Saxon.[228] Among High German dialects, Central German and East Franconian show evidence of reduction earliest, in the 9th century, with evidence for reduction in Old Saxon immediately following. Bavarian and Alemannic only show reduction later.[229][230] Alemannic appears to have been particularly conservative, reducing only unstressed short vowels to schwa while retaining distinct unstressed long vowels well into the Middle High German period. The farthest southern Highest Alemannic, such as Walser German, have retained distinct vowels in unstressed syllables to the present, such as nominative plural die Taga, genitive plural der Tago. The decreased isolation of these isolated mountain valleys in the 20th century has meant that the distinction between final vowels has become less clear here as well.[231][232]
Middle Dutch continued to have an unstressed long vowel (/iː/) in the ending -lijk and an unstressed short vowel (/i/) in the ending -ig. In modern Dutch, both endings have been reduced to schwa.[233]
Syncope and apocope
[edit]The loss of final unstressed vowels (usually schwa [ə]) forms a significant isogloss within West Germanic dialects. The so-called "Early New High German apocope" was a general loss of final /-e/ in all instances, which appeared first in Bavarian in the 13th century and came to encompass most Alemannic and West Central German, but not East Central German, which retained many instances of final schwa.[234] A similar change is independently attested in Hollandic Low Franconian, also in the 13th century; however, schwa was retained on some categories of words.[235] Apocope is not attested in Low German until the 16th century, when it appears in the north, around Mecklenburg.[236][237] Among the modern dialects, final schwa is retained in a continuous belt that comprises Low Saxon and High German dialects on both sides of the Benrath line and extends into the Dutch Low Saxon area, additionally in a relic area within Low Franconian in the southwest (West Flemish, Zeelandic, parts of East Flemish). It was lost to the north and south of this belt: in the northern part of Low Saxon, in much of West Central German and almost all of Upper German; however, the loss is in some instances morphologically conditioned, as, for plural morpheme -e is retained in a much larger area than the dative ending -e.[238][239][240][r] Some dialects, such as Low German on the border with Low Franconian and Thuringian, only apocopate schwa after sonorants, but retain it after stop consonants: strotə but bluəm (cf. standard German Blume).[242]
Beginning in Middle High German, most High German dialects and standard German have syncopated many unstressed vowels in final syllables, depending on the surrounding consonants: houbet > Haupt "head".[243] Middle Dutch shows a more advanced stage of syncope in unstressed final syllables than contemporary Middle High German, but tends to retain schwa in initial syllables.[244] Low German dialects vary in how much syncope they show: North Low German and East Low German have syncopated most medial unstressed syllables, whereas southern Westphalian and Eastphalian often have not, retaining unstressed vowels in words such as lengeste "longest".[245]
The deletion of schwa in unstressed prefixes such as ge- and be- depends on the dialect, with progressively more syncopation as one moves south. Low German, Low and Middle Franconian, Low and Upper Hessian, and East Central German generally do not syncopate prefixes at all, whereas Swabian, Upper Alsatian, Bavarian, and Swiss Alemannic generally syncopate schwa in all instances. East Franconian and Bavarian show the least prefix syncopation in the north, with it progressively increasingly to cover more consonant combinations as one moves south. Dialects in between syncopate to lesser amounts. However, some syncopated forms are found in almost all dialects and appear to be older, namely the syncopation of schwa before /r/ and /l/ in some lexemes, found in for instance German bleiben (< belîben) and glauben (< gelouben, cf. Dutch geloven).[246][247][248]
Svarabhakhti (vowel epenthesis)
[edit]The insertion of a vowel to break up a cluster is called vowel epenthesis or svarabhakti. It is common in High German and Dutch dialects, but is almost completely absent from Low German.[249]
Svarabhakti is found in all Dutch dialects except for northeastern ones (Dutch Low Saxon) and southwestern ones (Flanders, Overijssel). It has been present in Dutch since the Middle Dutch period, as reflected in frequent spellings with epenthetic e (e.g. arem.[250] A mild form of vowel epenthesis is also found in standard Dutch, in which a schwa is inserted especially to break up a cluster of /r l/ and a following labial or back consonant at the end of a syllable (e.g. arm, pronounced /arrəm/. It also appears in the combination /rn/, and, in Brabantine, in /gl/ or /gr/ at the beginning of a syllable (e.g. gəlas "glass").[251]
In High German dialects, either [ə] or [ɪ] is inserted to break up consonant clusters of r or l + a following labial or back consonant (e.g. Thuringian dorəf "village"). This feature is found in all High German dialects to varying degrees and can be traced back to the 13th century.[s] Epenthesis is not usually present in the cluster /rn/, but it is found in some dialects, e.g. in Lorraine Franconian ʃteːrən "star". Some dialects also experience epenthesis in the combination n + fricative consonant, e.g. South Bavarian hɔ̃ːnəf "hemp".[249]
Unrounding and secondary rounding
[edit]The majority of High German dialects have experienced the unrounding of the front rounded vowels /y/ (written <ü>) and /ø/ (written <ö>) to their unrounded counterparts /i/ and /e/ respectively.[253] The change probably began in the 13th century in Bavarian at the latest and then spread throughout the early modern period. Today, it includes all High German dialects except for Ripuarian, East Franconian, and High Alemannic.[254][t] Unrounded pronunciations of umlauted vowels were also formerly encountered as part of the High German standard until the 19th century, but are today considered nonstandard.[116][256]
Most Low German and Low Franconian dialects did not experience unrounding. It is found in Low German dialects from around Bremen and was previously present in some East Low German varieties spoken in what is now Poland and Russia's Kaliningrad Oblast.[257][u] In Low Franconian, systematic unrounding is mainly found in the southern part of the Brabantian area around Leuven and in some Central and West Limburgian varieties of South Low Franconian.[259][260][v]
There was also an opposite process, "secondary rounding," in which the front unrounded vowels /i/ and /e/ were rounded /y/ and /ø/ respectively. Secondary rounding is a fairly irregular process, usually occurring around /w/, other labial consonants, /ʃ/, and especially before /r/ and /l/.[263] It occurred very frequently in Low German dialects beginning in the 13th century, with the most secondary rounding taking place in North Low German. Commonly rounded forms in Low German include bün (German bin).[116][264] In the High German dialect area, it seems to have begun in the 13th century in the High Alemannic area, then spread to Swabian and East Franconian.[265] Among High German dialects, it is today especially common in High Alemannic, northern East Franconian, East Hessian, and Thuringian. e.g. dṻʃ (standard German Tisch). It is occasionally found even in dialects that have unrounded their umlauted vowels, such as Central and South Bavarian, e.g. Central Bavarian ṏfm (standard German Ofen).[266][267]
Spontaneous fronting
[edit]Spontaneous fronting of the rounded back vowels/diphthongs /o(ː), u(ː), ou, au, ua, uə/ occurred in Low Fraconian dialects and some High German dialects. Unlike i-umlaut, this fronting is not conditioned by the phonological environment. Spontaneous fronting can result in partially fronted rounded central vowels (e.g. /ʉ/, /ɤ/ etc.), or in fully fronted rounded vowels (/y/, /ø/ etc.).
Most Low Franconian dialects except for some in the Southeast have spontaneous fronting of ū to /y:/ (and thus merged it with original /y:/ that arose from the early West Germanic dipthong /iu/)[268], as shown by the change of earlier hūs to huis (/hyːs/ in Middle Dutch, diphthongized to /hœys/ in Modern Dutch). This palatalization also spread (often paired with shortening to /y/) to some Low Saxon dialects in the Netherlands.[269] Spontaneous palatalization of /u:/ did not occur in South Low Franconian (except in the westernmost part) and in the northeastern part of the Brabantian dialect area.[270][271] Sponaneous fronting of Middle Dutch ō (from Proto-Germanic *au) has a much more limited scope and only occurs in East Flanders and the southwesternmost part of Brabant (including Brussels), where it is realized as /yə/ (e.g. /byəm/ < bōm 'tree').[272][273]
In High German, spontaneous fronting is especially associated with High and Low Alemannic, North Hessian, and West Thuringian: e.g. Low Alemannic hüs ("house"), brüədər ("brother"). West Thuringian and Upper Saxon instead shift to unrounded back vowels vowels (/ʉ/, /ɤ/).[274] In Low German, fronting occurs most frequently before dental consonants. sünne ("sun"), dörp ("village", cf. High German Dorf). The process effected Westphalian the least and East Frisian Low German the most.[275]
In some Low Fraconian and High German and dialects, spontaneous fronting is linked to the unrounding of historical front rounded vowels. In these dialects, the resulting vowels re-occupy the place of the historical rounded front vowels that have undergone unrounding, which can be analysed as a chain shift, e.g. /uː/ > /yː/ > /iː/.[276][261] Spontaneous fronting rarely affects all rounded back vowels/diphthongs to the same degree. As a rule, /uː/, /ou/ and /uə/ are more prone to spontaneous fronting than /oː/ and short back vowels.[277][w]
Open syllable lengthening
[edit]Open syllable lengthening refers to the change of short vowels to long vowels in stressed open syllables. It is first attested in Low Franconian around 1200, and is theorized to have spread from there to High and Low German.[279][280][x]
In early Middle Dutch, open syllable lengthening lengthened /e a o/ in open syllables but lowered /i u/ to /e o/. In standard Dutch, these vowels have merged with /eː oː/, but Eastern Dutch dialects continue to distinguish between them.[282] Alterations in vowel length between different forms of the same word are mostly leveled out, with verbs and adjectives taking the short form, while nouns might take either. Several nouns have maintained the alteration between a short vowel in the singular and a long one in the plural: wĕg, plural we:gen, schĭp, plural sche:pen.[283] In modern Dutch dialects and standard Dutch, final schwa has often been lost, leaving the lengthened vowels in closed syllables (e.g. naam from naːme).[284]
Open syllable lengthening occurred early in Middle Low German as well. With the exception of Westphalian, where diphthongization had previously taken place in open syllables, Most Low German dialects follow the same pattern as Low Franconian, lowering long /i u y/ to /eː oː øː/.[285] In North Low German and East Low German, old /o e/ were further merged with /a/. In Eastphalian, by contrast, /u y/ were lowered to /o ø/, but /i/ lowered to /e/ and /e/ lowered to /ɛ/, thus keeping the vowels distinct. In Westphalian, however, diphthongization had previously taken place for vowels in open syllables. These short diphthongs were frequently lengthened; in northwestern Westphalian, they were not lengthened, however, and have frequently re-monophthongized. The area still retaining the short diphthongs has steadily decreased in size over time.[286] In southeastern Twente Dutch Low Saxon, short /i e o u/ do not lengthen except before v, g, z, and sometimes d, where they diphthongize as in neighboring Westphalian. This leaves short vowel in forms such as ěttn̩ "to eat", kŭkkn̩ "chick"; northeastern Twente has short vowels in all positions.[287] In Dutch Low Saxon spoken around Groningen, /i e/ do not lengthen in open syllables.[288]
Open syllable lengthening occurred somewhat later in High German dialects, spreading to West Central German soon after it began in Low Franconian, by the 13th century all of Central German, and by the 14th century most Upper German dialects.[281] In most dialects stressed short vowels in open syllables were lengthened in a more regular way than Dutch or Low German, without affecting the quality of the vowel: năme > Na:me, sĭgen > siegen (⟨ie⟩ = [i:]). Unlike in Dutch or Low German, in cases where a noun paradigm would thereby have different vowel lengths, typically the long vowel was chosen: thus We:g "way" with a long vowel because of the plural We:ge, but the adverb is wěg (away) with a short vowel.[289][290][y] Several Bavarian dialects lack the feature of leveling the paradigm, instead retaining short vowels in words like Weg, as do some High Alemannic dialects.[293] Additionally, open syllable lengthening did not affect many middle and southern Alemannic dialects, and there are isolated pockets in southern Bavarian, including the Cimbrian language island, which never acquired the feature.[294]
In High and Low German dialects, open syllable lengthening is often absent before t, m, and endings -er, -el, -en and -em in a following syllable.[z] In some West Central German dialects like Palatinate, these endings always prevented open syllable lengthening,[296] whereas in other High German dialects, the rule depends on a preceeding consonant (e.g. in east Central German, the lengthening is blocked only when the vowel is t, m, or p: Thuringian håmr "hammer", himl "heaven", but håːwr "oats", faːdr "feather".[297][aa] In Low German dialects, lengthening was prevented in fewer instances. In most dialects of Middle Low German besides Westphalian, o and e did not lengthen or were shortened again before t d m n and a following unstressed -er or -el ( hěmmel "heaven", bětter "better". Cf. the lengthened Dutch be:ter). In other instances, lengthened vowels are often found where High German has retained a short vowel, e.g. Hamburg North Low German reːdn "ridden".[299] In Eastphalian and to some extent other dialects, e and o are also found short before n d t before unstressed -en. Sometimes Eastphalian has a short vowel before other endings, such as -ing, -ich, or with the consonants g and p before the same unstressed final syllables (e.g. ǒppenen "to open" vs. other Low German oːpenen).[300][301] Among Dutch dialects, failure to lengthen is found in northern Brabantine, where it followed the same general rule of not lengthening vowels in open syllables before -el, -er, -en, -em.[288]
Closed Syllable shortening
[edit]Closed syllable shortening refers to a process in which long vowels and diphthongs were shortened in closed syllables. It is not as widespread or as regular as open syllable lengthening.[302]
Among High German dialects, closed syllable shortening occurred in Central German dialects in the 12th century. Bavarian shows only a few examples and Alemannic mostly lacks shortening. It occurred especially before /xt/ (e.g. gebra:ht > gebrǎcht "brought", lieht > Licht "light"), /rC/ (C=any consonant), before geminate or formerly geminate consonants (OHG geno:zz > MHG geno:z) > modern Genǒsse), and also in unstressed endings with long vowels like MHG -li:ch (modern standard German -lĭch).[302][303]
Middle Low German often shortened vowels before two consonants, although these shortenings were often removed again via analogy, especially in the northern East Low German dialects. They remain before /ft/ and /xt/, however.[301]
The northwestern dialects of Middle Dutch shared the shortening of long vowels and diphthongs before /xt/ with High and Low German (e.g. Dutch dǎcht, licht), which in Low Franconian can also come from earlier /ft/ (e.g. gekŏcht, from koːpen "to buy"). Occasional shortenings occurred before other vowel combinations (e.g. liːnden > lǐnden.[304][305] Some other dialects show shortening before m, e.g. blom vs. standard bloem.[306]
High vowel lowering
[edit]High vowel lowering refers to the lowering of high vowels such as i, ü, and u to e, ö and o. Typically, only short vowels are affected, though the lowering of long vowels is also attested (see the sections on Dutch and Low German in Open syllable lengthening). High vowel lowering is found in both standard High German and Dutch in many instances, as well as many of their dialects, most frequently around consonants such as n, m, l, and r. Among German dialects, it is most characteristic of Central German, and is more commonly found in West Central German than East Central German.[307][308]
The majority of Franconian dialects, both in Germany and the Netherlands, have undergone lowering of high vowels in at least some situations. In southwestern Dutch dialects and southern Hollandic, as well as standard Dutch, o and u in closed syllables merged to o in most instances: Dutch ons vs. German uns. In northern Dutch dialects, o from u and original o remain distinguished in spite of the lowering, with old u as [o] (traditionally spelled ⟨ó⟩ in Dutch dialectology) and old o as [ɔ] (traditionally spelled ⟨ò⟩).[309] The southwestern Dutch dialects also saw a lowering of i to e in closed syllables in the Middle Dutch period, however, this change has since been reversed in most instances, although southern dialects continue to pronounce a more open short /i/ and /e/ as [æ].[84][ab] The lowering is most consistent in the German Low Franconian and Central Franconian dialects, e.g. Low Franconian ek "I", loxt "air". In many Ripuarian and Moselle Franconian dialects, i was further lowered to [ɛ] or even [a], labialized to [o] or [ɔ], or rounded to [œ] or [ø]. Lowering is also common in Lower and Central Hessian, but with far more exceptions; it is likewise sporadically found in Rhine Franconian, and in sporadic examples in southern Hessian. The rules for when a lowering takes place vary throughout Central German and are often no longer obvious from the present-day. [308] It has also consistently occurred in Lower Alemannic Alsatian and in some varieties of Swabian. The only East Central German dialect that consistently has lowering is Thuringian.[311][307]
In Middle Dutch, lowering appears to have begun in the south and spread northwards.[312] Among the German dialects, the change can be dated to the 12th century.[307] It seems to have begun in Low and Central Franconian and spread from there. In the more eastern and southern West Central German dialects, the change has been largely been reversed under influence from Upper German, thus separating Alsatian from the other dialects that have the feature most consequently. It has likewise mostly been lost in Saxon, which only shows a few relict areas.[313]
/r/-coloring
[edit]The consonant /r/ frequently affects preceding consonants in continental West Germanic dialects. Frequent changes include raising or lowering as well as lengthening or shortening.
In Middle Dutch, short a, o, and ø (spelled ⟨eu⟩) were often lengthened before /r/, particularly before a dental consonant. This change is especially characteristic of western Low Franconian dialects and dates to the 13th century. The lengthening was accompanied by a decrease in the tenseness of the vowels. In southern dialects, uu also become more open, becoming [œː]. Limburgs sees a lowering of earlier ⟨oe⟩ (pronounced [uː]) to oː, e.g. zoor (cf. standard Dutch zoer, "sour"). [314][315][316] Other changes took place before particular consonants, with many dialects showing variation between different words as to how the vowel was changed; generally, an area that depalatalizes vowels can be observed to exist in the northeast, while an area that palatalizes them existed in the southern Brabantine.[317]
- In most dialects, -ir was lowered to -er before another consonant (e.g. kerk, berk vs. standard German Kirche Birke "church", "birch"), however Limburgs often still retains the original i (kirk, birk). The degree to which this change is consistent in Dutch dialects shows that it is older than many of the other vowel quality changes before r.[318]
- Old Low Franconian ar was lengthened before a dental: baart vs. German Bart "beard".[ac] Before a labial consonant except m, Old Dutch ar became er without lengthening: scarp > scherp "sharp".[316][315] In central southern dialects, namely Belgian Brabantine and eastern East Flemish, the shift ar to er was more general, including erm as well as before dental consonants zwert "black".[320] In West Flemish, ar was sometimes instead raised to or before m: West Flemish òòrm vs. standard Dutch arm "poor", but dèèrm vs. standard Dutch darm "intestine". The change to òò occurred without exception in Gentish West Flemish.[321]
- Old Low Franconian er is handled differently in different dialects. In the western coastal area, original er was lowered to ar before a dental consonant, often with lengthening (Old Dutch erda > aarde "earth").[ad] In northern dialects such as some northern Hollandic dialects, Stadsfries, and Utrechtish, er before a dental is lengthened rather than changing quality: peerd rather than standard Dutch paard "horse". However, in a large area, especially in North Brabant, Zeeland, and southern Holland, er became or before a dental, often with metathesis. In some northern Hollandic dialects, this occurred before before velar and labial consonants; in Zandvoortish, this has since become [œ], e.g. durde vs. standard derde "third.[322]
- -ur often lowered to -or before a dental consonant. In Brabantine, however, u was retained, creating doublets such as worm and wurm "worm".[316][315][314]
- Generally, o lengthened when before r and a dental: doorn vs. Dorn "thorn" In Hollandic, -or often palatalized to œr (spelled ⟨ur⟩ before labial consonants, with forms such as durp "village" being common. Such forms are also found in modern West Flemish and French Flemish, including before dental and labial consonants.[323][315]
Low German experienced lowering of high vowels before r and a following labial or velar consonant in the 11th century.[324] In Eastphalian, North Low German, and East Low German, -er was also lowered to -ar and -ir to -er or further to -ar, giving forms such as berke, barg for Old Saxon birke, berg. A general lengthening of a short vowel before /r/ and a following consonant is found in North and East Low German, giving forms such as woːrm "worm", döːrp "village".[301][301]
In Central German, the sequence of Vr + dental has variable outcomes: sometimes it results in vowel lengthening with a vocalization of /r/ (e.g. Central Hessian boːɐd vs. standard German Bord), and sometimes it results in vowel shortening and the loss of the /r/ (e.g. Central Hessian ʃwǫds vs. standard German schwarz). Lengthening of the vowel in the sequence before velar and liquid consonants are rare.[325] In both High and Low German dialects, the present of /r/ is a frequent cause of secondary rounding as well.[263]
/r/-vocalization
[edit]In many Low German, Franconian, and Central Bavarian dialects, /r/ commonly vocalizes to a central vowel before a consonant or word-finally: generally to /ɐ/ or /ʌ/ in southern and northern dialects and to /ə/ in western Central German. The degree and regularity of this change varies widely from place to place and dialect to dialect. In Alemannic, parts of the Swabian and Appenzellerish instead lose syllable final /r/ with compensatory lengthening. In most Low German and Central German dialects, /r/ is most often vocalized before a dental consonant, but retained in other positions.[326]
/l/-coloring and vocalization
[edit]Continental West Germanic shows two large areas with vocalization of earlier /l/. This change is usually associated with a velar pronunciation of /l/ ([ɫ]), which can also effect the quality of preceding vowels.[327]
Velar pronunciation of /l/ is common in Low German and Dutch Low Saxon dialects, especially before /d t/. This has had the effect of labializing and lengthening /a/: Old Saxon ald > Middle Low German ōld. Many Low German dialects have developed a diphthongal pronunciation in this environment, e.g. Eastphalian åult "old".[327] Complete vocalization of /l/ to /u/ is frequently found in western continental West Germanic dialects, including western Swiss Alemannic and in Central Franconian dialects north of Aachen. In Dutch, the vocalization only occurred after /o/, which could result from from earlier /al/, /ol/, and /ul/ (the latter via lowering) before /d t/. The outcome varies: whereas West and French Flemish has uː, other Dutch dialects vary between [ɔu] (as in standard Dutch),[ae] [æu], and [æi]. Limburgs does not merge a, o, and u, and features both vocalized and non-vocalized /l/ after all three vowels before /d t/, depending on dialect.[329] In the High German dialects, the conditions for vocalization vary. Some restrict it to only before a dental stop, as in Dutch, in others it occurs word finally, between vowels, and with geminates, e.g. Swiss German sau̯ts "salt", taːu̯ "valley" (German Tal), ɤugu "bird (German Vogel), zɔu̯ɘ "sole", tæu̯u̯ɘr "plate" (German Teller).[330] Vocalizations across this area are not all part of the same process. They occurred at different times, with Dutch dialects showing spelling such as goud (cf. standard German Gold) already in 11th century, whereas the Swiss German vocalization probably occurred no earlier than the 18th century.[331]
The second large area, Central Bavarian, shows a different form of vocalization, to /y/ (German ⟨ü⟩) or /i/, although here vocalization to /u/ is also found. This change begins in the 14th century: Middle High German /l/ is vocalization before a consonant or at the end of a word, producing a vowel more similar to /y/ in eastern dialects and more similar to /u/ in western ones. In the east, this led to the production of new rounded vowel phonemes in combinations such as MHG /i/ + /l/ =/y/, e.g. wyd vs. MHG wild, MHG /e/ + /l/ = /ø/, e.g. ʃdøn vs. MHG stellen, MHG /ë/ + /l/ = /œː/, e.g. ʃdœːn vs. steln, and MHG /î/ + /l/ = /ay/, e.g. way vs. MHG wîl(e). In Western Central Bavarian dialects, these combinations instead produce /iː/, /ei/, /ɛi/, and /ai/.[332][330]
Some dialects of Eastern Pomeranian Low German shows a unique form of vocalization. Here, Middle Low German final /l/ is vocalized to /ɐ/, e.g. faiɐ (cf. High German viel). This change may derive from Slavic influence.[333][af]
Franconian tone accent
[edit]In the western part of the continental West Germanic dialect continuum, three dialect groups, viz. South Low Franconian, Ripuarian and Moselle Franconian, have developed contrastive tone accent on stressed long vowels, diphthongs and short vowels followed by a sonorant (i.e. /r, l, m, n, ŋ/). Two types of tone are distinguished which are conventionally called Stoßton/stoottoon ('thrusting tone') or simply tone accent 1 (TA 1) and Schleifton/sleeptoon ('slurring tone') or tone accent 2 (TA 2). While indivdual dialects may differ in detail, TA 1 is for the most part realized (in declarative sentences) as a strong pitch drop (= falling tone) within the syllable, while TA 2 is usually realized as a level high tone or a high falling tone with only a slight pitch drop.[334][335]
While the phonetic driving force that caused the development of tone accent and its geographicial origin are still a matter of debate, the historical phonological conditions of it are well understood. The tone accent of a syllable in modern dialects depends on the original quantity and quality of the stressed vowel in Middle High German and Middle Limburgian (= Middle East Low Franconian):
- Most Ripuarian and Moselle Franconian follow the so-called "Rule A". Under Rule A, originally long mid and low vowels (i.e. /eː, ɛː, oː, ɔː, aː/) and falling diphthongs (/ie, uo/) always produced TA 1 (e.g. skaːf > Ripuarian ʃɔː¹f 'sheep'). High long vowels (/iː, uː/), rising diphthongs (/ei, ou/) and originally short vowels that underwent open-syllable lengthening mostly received TA 2 (e.g. muːs > muː²s 'mouse'; maxən > maː²xə 'make'); however, in the special case when these vowels/diphthongs were followed by an unstressed syllable that started in a voiced consonant, they developed TA 1 (e.g. bliːbən > bliː¹və 'stay').[336][337]
- South Low Franconian mostly follow "Rule A2", which is largely identical to Rule A but additionally requires that the unstressed vowel following a voiced consonant undergoes apocope in order to trigger TA 1 for high long vowels, rising diphthongs, and lengthened short vowels. For instance, the Sittard dialect has banə > baː¹n 'road' where unstressed /ə/ underwent apocope.[338][339] The difference between Rule A and Rule A2 is only visible when unstressed /ə/ following a voiced consonant did not undergo apocope: TA 1 with high vowels and rising diphthongs is then triggered only in Rule A dialects, but not in Rule A2 dialects. For instance, skriːbən becomes ʃriː¹və in Kölsch (Ripuarian, Rule A), but ʃriː²və in the Central Limburgian dialect of Maasbracht (South Low Franconian, Rule A2).[340]
- Finally, "Rule B" is observed in the southeastern part of the Moselle Franconian dialect area. Rule B largely operates as a mirror image of Rule A, with high long vowels and rising diphthongs receiving TA 1, while long non-high vowels and falling diphthongs have TA 2.[338][339]
Northern Low German has developed a phonologically similar feature, known as Knick ("bend").[341]
Grammar
[edit]Pronouns
[edit]Modern standard Dutch shows no distinction between accusative and dative pronouns for any person, and sometimes no longer distinguishes between subject and object forms. Some dialects retain a distinction, or did so until recently, for the third person pronouns. Others have simplified the system further, using for instance ons ("us") as a subject pronoun.[342]
1st and 2nd person singular pronouns
[edit]Proto-Germanic featured a pronominal system for the first and second person singular ("I", "you") featuring four distinct forms, a nominative, an accusative, a dative, and a genitive/possessive. All three dialect areas have maintained the nominative form for "I", variously ich (High German) or ik (Dutch, Low German). The forms of the 2nd person singular vary more widely as most Dutch dialects have replaced the earlier pronoun du, still used in High and Low German, with the plural form, je; the original du is found today only on the western edge of the Low Franconian dialect area.[343][344][345]
In West Germanic, North Sea Germanic tends to lose the distinction between accusative and dative forms of the 1st and 2nd person singular pronouns ("me", "you").[346][347] Most Low German dialects level in favor of the dative (mi and di), but some, such as Eastphalian, instead favor the original accusative (e.g. mek, deck). The loss of distinct accusative/dative pronouns for the first and second person singular had already occurred in Old Saxon, although accusative forms are occasionally attested, and they resurface as general forms in some dialects of Middle Low German.[348][349] Middle Dutch likewise attests a loss of the distinction in favor of the dative, giving mi (modern mij and di, although some High German influenced originally accusative forms (mik/mich) are found in the southeast and in dialects bordering Germany.[350] Most High German dialects maintain a distinction, having mich dich (acc.) and mir dir (dat.).[351][352]
For "you", modern standard Dutch uses subject jij, object form jou, with an unstressed form je that can be used for either form. In the south, the pronoun begins with /g-/ rather than /j-/ (gij). While these are originally plural forms, related to German ihr and Low German jy (see below), in modern Dutch, they are only used in the singular except for je.[344][353]
1st and 2nd person plural pronouns
[edit]The first consonant of the nominative/subject 1st person plural pronoun varies throughout the dialect continuum. Within Southeastern Low Franconian (? unclear from source and my Lower Rhenish geography is not the best), forms with initial f (fer/vir/för/fir) are found east of Eupen, around Geilenkirchen and Erkelenz, as well as east of Dülken, Süchteln, and around Remscheid. Initial f is also found in a large around of neighboring Low German Westphalian (fi/fui/feï). The remainder of Low German has initial w. Dialects south of the ik/ich line have initial m. (?).[354]
Low German and Dutch have inherited the initial /j/ of the second person plural pronoun; in High German dialects, the sound has been lost (Dutch jij, Low German ji vs. High German ihr.[355]
Proto-West Germanic probably made no distinction between accusative and dative for the 1st and 2nd person plural ("us", "you"), and almost all modern High and Low German dialects have a single pronoun for both. In Old High German, however, separate accusative and dative forms were innovated: unsih, iuwih (accusative) vs. uns, iu (dative). Low Saxon also vary rarely attests a form unsik, while a form jüch (as opposed to dative jü) is attested early in North Middle Low German.[ag] In High German, the two forms were eventually leveled in favor of only uns for the 1st person and only euch for the second person.[356][357][358] Low German dialects vary as to which form they chose; Eastphalian uses the former accusative forms for both (üsch and jück), while other dialects may use the dative (us or uns for "us", ju for "you"). The small Westphalian dialect of Drolshagen-Plettenberg-Schmallenberg is unique among High and Low German dialects in having distinct dative and accusative pronouns for the 2nd person plural (auk [acc], au [dat]).[359][360]
Both Central and Upper Bavarian dialects (Upper German) and the Low German spoken around Dortmund have replaced the 2nd person plural pronoun with what was originally a dual form: nominative Bav. eß, Dort. (g)it, accusative/dative Bav. enk, Dort. ink. In modern Bavarian, these pronouns have mostly been ousted in favor of the more standard German ir/aich, except in rural areas.[361][362]
3rd person pronouns
[edit]With the exception of the High German dialects, all continental West Germanic dialects share the innovation of using a 3rd person masculine nominative/subject pronoun ("he") beginning with h- (Dutch hij, Low German he vs. High German er).[363] h- forms are also found in the Central German Rhineland dialects; there is also a transition zone with a mixed form her.[364]
Reflexive pronoun
[edit]Low German and Low Franconian had originally lost their third person reflexive pronouns (that is, there was no special word for himself as opposed to him), a feature shared with the North Sea Germanic languages English and Frisian.[365] More recently, the High German reflexive pronoun sich has been imported into both Low German and standard Dutch.[366] Southwestern, Northwestern, and most Eastern Low Franconian dialects continue to use the same pronoun for both "him" and "himself;" eastern dialects near the German border also use the forms zik, zich, or zich.[367]
Verbs
[edit]First person singular ending
[edit]Most Proto-West Germanic verbs used a vocalic ending to mark the first person singular, variously -u and -ō, depending on verb class (e.g. OHG/OS biru, "I bear", OHG/OS neriu "I nourish") [368] Old High German and Old Saxon the first person singular ending-m from the athematic verbs to some classes of weak verbs: in Old Saxon and Old High German it appears on verbs with the vowel -o/ō (e.g. OS salbon, OHG salbōm "I salve"), while Old High German also extended it to weak verbs ending in -e/ē (e.g. OHG habēm vs. OS hebbiu "I have").[369]
In the High medieval period, the different verb classes leveled the first person singular in all continental Germanic dialects, mostly to -e (/-ə/) (the phonetic development of the vocalic ending). In dialects with apocope, this final schwa was subsequently lost. In modern Dutch dialects, the schwa is only retained in Zeelandic, Flemish, and Dutch Low Saxon.[370] Across the Dutch–German border, -e is retained in the western and southern parts of the Low German dialect area.[371] Among High German dialects, it is still found e.g. in Northern Hessian and the northern Thuringian dialects.[372]
In Middle Franconian and West Alemmanic, the leveling was instead to -en, the phonetic development of -m.[373] Various West Central German dialects, including all of Central Franconian, have retained this ending for the first person singular until the present (e.g. Luxembourgish ech kafen "I buy"), but the ending is often reduced to /-ə/ in many (or all) phonetic environments, depending on dialect.[374][375][376][377] While the Old Low Franconian Psalms use -en for all persons, most Middle Dutch shows the same leveling to -e as most Middle High German, with -en only appearing in Eastern dialects dialects (e.g. ic bidden "I ask").[378][379] The modern dialects of Zeeland and West Flanders feature a more general -en suffix for the first person singular. This form is probably not related to the earlier -en ending in older Low Franconian, but is rather the result of a more recent change influenced by the athematic verbs.[380][381]
The Dutch spoken in southern Holland and around Utrecht instead has a first person singular ending -t (e.g. ik geeft "I give").[382] This is part of the phenomenon of Eenheidssingularis ("unity singular") in these dialects, in which all singular verbs take the ending -t. This feature arose because the ending is lost when the verb and pronoun are inverted (cf. geef hij "does he give"), thus matching the 1st person singular form of the verb with schwa apocope.[383]
Second person singular ending
[edit]The earliest second person singular ending in Germanic was -Vs; in Old High German, this ending began to receive a final t, becoming -est in Middle High German. Typically, this change is argued to derive from a reanalysis of a following pronoun du as part of the verb stem: zelis dû > zelistû "you tell".[ah] Analogy with verbs already ending in -t like bist may have also played a role.[385]
In Middle High German, Central Franconian maintained the older -es ending, whereas in East Central German and Upper German, -est became more and more dominant.[386] Some Central Franconian dialects continue to feature a verb ending -s, e.g. Cologne du nimps "you take".[375] The final -t has been subsequently lost in some dialects, such as south Palatinate, South Franconian, Alsatian, some Swiss German dialects, and rarely in Swabian. In those dialects where -st became -ʃt, this leaves an ending -ʃ, as in South Franconian ʃdaikʃ "you climb". In some Ripuarian and German Low Franconian dialects, the final -t is lost only after voiceless consonants, e.g. Bergish jriːfs "you grab".[387]
In Middle Dutch, while the form -(e)st does occur, it is not as common.[388] In the modern Netherlands, most dialects have lost the second person singular pronoun doe. Among those that maintain it, distinctive second person singular verb endings remain. In East Limburgish and Dutch Low German near the German border near Groningen and Twente, the form -s is still maintained without a following -t. In Groningen itself, both -ste and the simplification -se appear.[389] Final -s is also maintained in German Low Franconian. In German Low German, the ending -(e)s without following -t is still found in Westphalian and western Holstein,[390][391] while other Low German dialects adopted the form -(e)st during the Middle Low German period.[392] Some of these have subsequently lost the -t because of phonetic rules, e.g. Emslandish ɣrips "you grab" but ziŋkst "you sing".[393]
Syncope in verb endings
[edit]As a result of schwa syncope, most modern continental German dialects have reduced the verb endings -et, -es(t), and -ete/-ede to -t, -s(t), and -te/-de.
Most Westphalian and parts of Eastphalian do not syncopate the schwa in the endings -est, -et (in Low German, 3rd person singular and all plural), or -ede. Other Low German dialects syncopated these endings during the Middle Low German period. The North Low German of Emsland losses the -t entirely in verbs stems ending in stops or fricatives: fänk "catches" and drif "drives".[394]
Standard German retains the schwa in these endings when the verb stem ends in a dental stop (/d/ or /t/), e.g. er redet.[263] An exception is made for the 2nd and 3rd person singular present of strong verbs with a vowel change (e.g. 3rd person singular er wird, er rät, but 2nd person plural ihr werdet, ihr ratet).[395] The syncope on these verbs predates that of schwa more generally, already being common in Middle High German.[396] For other verbs, Early New High German still shows inconsistent application of syncope.[243] Virtually all modern High German dialects syncopate the vowel even when the stem ends in a dental stop, leading to forms such as er fint (rather than er findet) and er ret (rather than er redet), geredt (rather than geredet). On the other hand, some southern High German dialects use unsyncopated forms where the standard has adopted a syncopated form, e.g. er ratet (rather than er rät).[397]
Syncope of schwa in verb endings was already common in Middle Dutch.[388] Standard Dutch and most dialects syncopate the schwa even when the stem ends in a dental stop (e.g. hij vindt "he finds"), however some Hollandic, Zeelandic, and French-Flemish dialects maintain the schwa before dental stops. The dialect of Stellingwerfs also maintains the schwa after b (e.g. schabbet), while the dialect of Goeree-Overflakkee maintains the long preterite form -ede.[398]
Plural endings
[edit]Proto-West Germanic featured distinct endings for the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd person plural.[ai] Among modern dialects, three distinct endings are only retained in the southern part of Highest Alemannic in Valais and in some Central Bavarian dialects (Niederbayern, parts of Austria),[401] e.g. Walser German wir mache, ir machet, schi machunt.[402] Ripuarian maintains three forms only for athematic verbs.[403] All other dialects have simplified the system in some way.
Old Saxon, in common with other North Sea Germanic languages but unlike Old High German or Old Low Franconian, possessed a single ending for the plural (Einheitsplural), -ad. The subjunctive plural used -en.[9][404] In the Middle Low German period, dialects began to use a single form for the subjunctive and indicative. The former subjunctive ending -en came to be used as the general form especially in the east and, including in the literary language of the Hanseatic League. A third form, the suffix -ent, is found in Westphalian texts from the 13th and 14th century.[405][406] Most dialects in the western part of the modern Low Saxon area continue to use -(e)t, while dialects in the eastern part have adopted the subjunctive ending -(e)n; based on this feature, the latter are classified as Eastern Low German in traditional German dialectology. The ending -(e)n is also employed in the northernmost dialects in Schleswig; in the northwest on both sides of the Dutch–German border in the Stellingwerven, Groningen, and East Friesland; in the Waldeck region around Korbach bordering on northern Hessian; and in a narrow stretch of Westphalian dialects in the southwest close to the boundary with the Ripuarian dialect area.[aj][408] The suffix -ent survives in a small pocket of southern Westphalian dialects around Olpe and Drolshagen.[9][ak]
Most Low Franconian and High German dialects have conflated the first and third person plural endings (-en), but retain a distinct ending for the second person plural (-t).[409] The simplification of -ent to -en can be dated to before 1350 in Central German and spread from there into Bavarian and East Franconian by the 15th century, while it predates the Middle Dutch period (1250) in Low Franconian.[410][411] In many Low Franconian varieties (including standard Dutch), the historical second person plural form has acquired a singular function (e.g. standard Dutch jij maakt 'you (sg.) make'), and a new plural has formed, taking the ending -en. This change has produced a secondary unitary plural that is historically unrelated to the Low Saxon unitary plural.[412] The ending -nt is retained in some South Low Franconian dialects in the first and third person plural forms of athematic and contracted verbs.[413]
Independent from Low Saxon, Einheitsplural also developed in High German dialects of the Southwest, affecting much of Alemannic and some adjacent Rhine Franconian dialects. In Rhine Franconian dialects west of the Rhine and in Low Alemannic, the general plural suffix is /-ə/ (from earlier -ən with n-apocope), while in Swabian and in the eastern part of the High Alemannic area the suffix /-ət/ (or /-əd/) is used, e.g. Low Alemannic mache vs. Swabian machet '(we/you/they) make', with contracted and athematic verbs instead taking either -n or -nt depending on dialect.[414] Highest Alemanic and western High Alemannic dialects still retain two or three distict plural verb endings.[415]
Preterite and perfect forms
[edit]Conjugation
[edit]West Limburgs has generalized the ending -n to all persons in the preterite.[416] The same change has occurred in the Ripuarian and some Moselle Franconian dialects. In some dialects, the use of this -n is limited to only certain types of verbs. In Bergish, it is limited to only weak verbs without Rückumlaut, whereas in northern Moselle Franconian, it is found on all verbs.[417]
Strong verbs
[edit]Most dialects have leveled the earlier ablaut between the singular and plural of strong verbs.[citation needed]
In Dutch dialects, distinct vowels between the singular and plural for at least some ablaut classes are maintained in Western Flemish around Herlen, in Belgian Eastern Limburgish, and in the Zeelandic spoken in northern Brabant.[418]
During the Middle Low German period, the umlauted vowel formerly used in the past subjunctive frequently came to replace the non-umlauted vowel of the past plural of strong verbs. North and East Low German have subsequently replaced this vowel with that of the preterite singular. However, Westphalian and some Eastphalian dialects have maintained the change in vowels: Westphalian ik halp "I helped", du hülpest "you (s) helped, weu hülpen "we help". The area of Osnabrück has leveled this difference by applying the vowel of the plural to the singular.[419][394]
Weak verbs
[edit]The preterite in Dutch dialects shows several forms. Some Hollandish, Zealandic, and French Flemish dialects add the full form -ede (e.g. ik bakkede "I baked"), at least to some verbs.[420] In most dialects with syncope of the "binding e" in old -ede, the form -de is assimilated to a preceding voiced consonant (e.g. ik bakte), as in standard Dutch.[421] However, most Limburgish dialects always adds -de, without devoicing.[416] South East Limburgish instead adds adds -et-, to which personal endings are then added (e.g. ich gaapet).[420] West Flemish adds -d/-t, a form that features both syncope and apocope, but French Flemish usually adds -ste (e.g. ik bakste "I baked), a form borrowed from the preterite present verbs (cf. archaic onste, dorste). East Flemish shows an expanded form -dege/-tege. This was originally a double ending -dede/-tede, with the second /d/ weakened to /ɤ/. Overflakkee, Schouwen-Duiveland, and Zeelandic-Flemish have a preterite ending -ende.[416]
In most of Low German, because of the loss of medial /d/, the ending -ede becomes -e (which can be apocopated): hör(e) "I hear/heard", hörest "you hear/heard", but höret "he hears" vs. hör(e) "he heard".[422] Dialects with this feature often use the perfect to express the 1st and 2nd person singular.[423] Westphalian, however, has retained medial /d/, sometimes with syncope of the preceding schwa: ik kuakede "I cooked", ik woende "I lived". After voiceless consonants, this syncopated -de becomes -te, a feature also of Dutch Low Saxon. In West Low German Neumarkish, this -te form has spread to other verbs, possibly under High German influenced.[422][421] Many West Low German dialects show rhoticism of medial /d/ to /r/, giving forms such as ik harre "I had".[423]
Weak verbs with "Rückumlaut"
[edit]Most Low German dialects have reduced the amount of "Rückumlaut", but in Westphalian (as in the neighboring West Central German) the verbs featuring "Rückumlaut" are frequent: e.g. setten - sat "set - set", seggen - sachte.[422] Rückumlaut is also common in the neighboring Limburgish dialects (e.g. heure "hear" [present tense] vs. ik hoort "I heard").[420]
Loss of the preterite
[edit]ge- prefix
[edit]By around the year 1000, the Proto-Germanic prefix *ga- had become grammaticalized with the past participle of verbs in continental West Germanic, around the same period that saw the development of the periphrastic perfect tense. As such, it is found in modern standard German and Dutch. Originally, the prefix seems to have had perfective meaning; its application to participles in the high medieval period was inconsistent. In the oldest stages of the continental West Germanic languages, the prefix was still perfective, and thus was not added to verbs that were themselves perfective or telic in meaning. Additionally, the prefix could be added to finite verbs to indicate pluperfect or future meaning, creating oppositions such as sah ("saw") vs. gisah ("came to see").[424][425]
In the Middle High German and Middle Dutch period, there remained a class of verbs that were felt to be inherently perfective and thus lacked the ge- prefix for their participles, such as gehen/gaan, werden/worden, komen, and vinden. In modern standard German and Dutch, the prefix is now added to most verbs lacking initial unstressed prefixes, with the difference that Dutch also adds them to verbs containing the suffix -eeren whereas verbs in standard German with -ieren are suffixless (Dutch gestudeerd vs. German studiert).[424][426] Several Low Franconian, Central, and Upper German dialects have retained the original practice of not adding ge- to certain verbs that are felt to be perfective, while standard German still does not add ge- to werden in the passive.[427]
Ge- has often been phonologically reduced or lost in modern West Germanic languages.[428] Central German retains the prefix in all instances, but in certain Upper German dialects of Bavarian, Alemmanic, and East Franconian, the vowel of the prefix is syncopated in front of certain consonants such as fricatives, nasals, semivowels, liquids, rhotics and /h/, giving g- (e.g. gflogə, cf. standard German geflogen). Moreover, many Upper German dialects delete ge- completely on verbs with stems beginning with a stop: *gbråxt > bråxt (standard German gebracht).[429][430] Northern Dutch dialects lack the ge-prefix entirely, while some more southerly ones reduced it to e-; far southern dialects and standard Dutch both retain ge- and it has been reintroduced further north at least partially through the influence of standard Dutch.[367] West Low German dialects show a similar development. The ge-prefix is lost in most of Westphalia and North Low German, but is retained in Eastphalian as e-.[431] Among East Low German dialects, Brandenburgian and Middle Pomeranian, northern dialects lack the prefix, central ones have e-, and southern ones have je-.[432] The largely extinct Low Prussian likewise had je-.[333]
Stem changes in the present tense
[edit]Early German languages attest both "height harmony" ("u-umlaut") and i-umlaut ("Germanic umlaut") in the verb paradigm of strong verbs in the present tense. The first person singular ending -u in Old High German and Old Saxon raised a previous e to i, as in nimu "I take". The 2nd person singular ending -is and the third person singular ending -id/-it caused both raising of e to i (as in nimis "you take", nimit "he takes") and umlaut of a to e (as in feris "you travel", ferit "he travels").[385] West Germanic *eu was also affected by "height harmony", raising to iu before -u, -is, and -id/-it.[citation needed] Different dialects have removed or retained this alterations to different extents.
- ie (<*eo) and eu (<*iu):
In Dutch, the variation was lost already in the Middle Dutch period.[433] Most High German dialects have also lost the variation. It is still found in West Central German (Ripuarian, some Upper Hessian dialects) and neighboring German Low Franconian, e.g. German Low Franconian ʃiːt "I shoot" but ʃyːts "you (s) shoot". In Upper German, the form is maintained most fully in Swabian, which has iu for the entire singular and the imperative (e.g. fliəgə "to fly", 1ps fluig "I fly", 2ps fluigʃd "you fly", 3ps fluigd "he flies", imper. fluig "fly!"). Some forms are also found in East Franconian and neighboring North Bavarian, as do some Swiss German dialects.[434] Most Low German dialects have maintained this vowel change (Middle Low German ē4 - ü), but restrict it to the 2nd and 3rd person singular.[435]
In Middle Dutch, open syllable lengthening turned short i in an open syllable into ē - this process has removed e/i variation in Dutch verb paradigms (e.g. Middle Dutch spreken, spreket vs. syncopated sprict). Other examples were removed via analogy.[436]
Subjunctive
[edit]Proto-West Germanic featured two subjunctive forms: a subjunctive made from the present tense stem ("present subjunctive") and a subjunctive made from the preterite tense stem ("past subjunctive"). [al]
Present subjunctive
[edit]In Proto-West Germanic, the present subjunctive featured an ending -ē for strong verbs and class I and III weak verbs, but an ending -ō for class II weak verbs. Verbs in the subjunctive lacked the third-person singular ending -Vþ found on indicative verbs.[438] In Old High German, this led to forms such as nëme "let him take".[439]
Today, present subjunctive forms are only found in Standard German and the Alemannic dialects, where they are used to indicate indirect speech, as well as in optative and jussive statements.[437] In High Alemannic, present subjunctive forms of all verbs are used, but "to have" and "to be" are especially common; "to have" and "to be" also have special subjunctive I forms in Lower Alemannic dialects. The most common use is to indicate a wish (e.g. High Alemannic I begäre, das er chömm "I want him to come", with the verb chömm in present subjunctive), while the meaning of possibility is limited to southern Alemannic.[440]
Past subjunctive
[edit]In Proto-West Germanic, the past subjunctive was formed by adding a vowel -ī to the preterite stem (including the dental ending of weak verbs), to which further endings for person and number could be added.[441] The past subjunctive of weak verbs did not experience umlaut, despite formerly having ended in -ī. Once final -ī reduced to schwa, this resulted in the merger of the preterite and past subjunctive of most weak verbs in all three dialect groups: hôrta "I heard" vs. hôrti "I would hear" > ich hôrte "I heard/would hear".[442][443] In High and Low German, the final -ī resulted in the umlaut of the subjunctive forms of strong verbs: nāmi > ich nǣme "I would take".[444][445]
In Upper Alemannic and Bavarian dialects (but not in East Franconian, Upper Alsatian, or Upper Swabian), the distinction between indicative and subjective verbs has been strengthened. Frequently, this has been done by vowel changes, including by using the umlauted vowel /ę/ or the unumlauted vowel /u/. This umlaut is not the only way of marking the subjunctive, however.[446] In Upper German dialects that have lost the preterite, the preterite form has sometimes been retained in a subjunctive meaning.[447] Bavarian, furthermore, has generalized the old dental preterite marker -ad into a general marker of the "past subjunctive" for all verbs, including for strong verbs (e.g. kàmad "would come") and model verbs (e.g. kuntad "could").[448] High Alemannic moves some weak verbs to the strong class in the subjunctive, but also vice versa: e.g. Bernese German miech or miechti "would make" from weak mache "to make", but also chämti "would come" from strong choo "come". Most frequently, the subjunctive involves changing the stem vowel of the verb to /ie/ or /ää/.[449]
Among West Central dialects, Luxemburgish has retained about 30 verbs with both a preterite and a "past subjunctive" form. Verbs are frequently differentiated by a vowel change, e.g. from ou (preterite) to éi (subjunctive).[450]
Most Low German and Low Franconian dialects have lost a distinctive past subjunctive. Most Low Franconian dialects only experienced umlaut of short a; as a consequence, the preterite and the past subjunctive stem were identical for almost all verbs, especially following the syncope of final schwa (gave > gaf).[451] A separate past subjunctive can still be found in Westflemish and Geldric. In Low German, the umlauted vowel of the strong subjunctive came to replace the unumlauted vowel of the preterite plural of weak verbs, from there also replacing the singular vowel in most dialects.[452] While subjunctives can be found in some other dialect groups, they are most frequent in Eastphalian and Westphalian, which maintain two distinct vowels in the preterite. Here, as in most Low Franconian and Low German dialects, the subjunctive remains distinct in only a few forms: here, the 1st and 3rd person singular: ik sünge "I would help" vs. ik sang "I sang.[453][454]
Athematic verbs and "short verbs"
[edit]Proto-West Germanic had a small class of 4 athematic verbs, descended from the Proto-Indo-European -mi verbs, that took the ending -m for the first person singular in the present tense. These appear in Old High German as stām "I stand", gām "I go", tōm/tuom "I do", and bim "I am".Aside from the third person in -m and the lack of vowel in the suffix, these verbs did not show any other unusual conjugation features in the present tense.[455] However, all of these verbs were suppletive to various degrees, especially in the non-present forms. bim in particular used three different roots, while gām and stām had longer forms, in Old High German gangan and stantan, and tōm shows reduplication in the preterite.[456][457][458]
In 11th-century High German, a new class of "short verbs" developed in the 11th century from the athematic verbs and six other verbs that were shortened to a single syllable, namely vân "to catch", slân "to hit, sên "to see", zien "to pull", hân "to have", and lân "to let". When used in their contracted forms, all of these verbs lacked a vowel in the verb endings and had a first person singular ending -n (the phonological development of the earlier -m). Analogies to the verbs gân and stân are visible throughout the paradigms of these verbs in the Middle High German period.[459][460][461] Except for the athematic verbs, longer, uncontracted forms also existed for the contracted verbs, and these uncontracted forms were encountered more frequently in certain persons and numbers (e.g. singular vs. plural), though this varied from verb to verb and from dialect to dialect. The most frequently contracted verbs were hân and lân.[462] Middle Dutch also shows a form ic haen "I have" as a contraction from haven, an alternative form of the verb hebben "to have".[463]
Standard German and many dialects have removed most of the contracted forms and expanded the former short verbs gên and stên to gehen and stehen, probably on analogy with sên > sehen.[464] Standard modern German only maintains two "short verbs", sein, "to be", which is highly suppletive, and tun "to do". Only "to be" maintains a final -n (ich bin "I am").[465] The verb haben, on the other hand, has contracted forms in the 2nd and 3rd person singular and preterite, but otherwise uses the uncontracted stem.[466]
"Short verbs" as a special class
[edit]Central Franconian has, depending on dialect, between six and seven "short verbs" with a unique conjugation, in Ripuarian han "to have", jon "to go", sin "to be", sin "to see", ston "to stand", and dun "to do". Luxembourgish adds ginn "to give".[am] In Ripuarian, only "short verbs" such as /doːn/ 'do', /jɔːn/ 'go', /ʃtɔːn/ 'stand', /zen/ 'be', and /han/ 'have' show the final /-n/ ending of the Central Franconian first person singular (e.g. jon "I go"). Additionally, they have distinct endings for all three persons in the plural: /miːʁ jɔːn/ "we go", /iːʁ jɔːt/ "you go", /ze jɔːnt/ "they go".[468][403] The neighboring South Low Franconian dialects (Limburgish) likewise show an -n ending for short verbs with stem final vowels in the first person singular (e.g. iχ ɣaːn "I go"). For the plural, most dialects have an ending -n for the 1st and 3rd person plural (wi-zi ɣaːn "we-they go"). Dialects in the far southeast instead have the ending -nt (wi-zi ɣaːnt "we-they go", wi-zi haːnt "we-they have").[413][469]
In Alemmanic, the "short verb" class includes up to 13 verbs. Besides those already mentioned, Alemmanic also includes gëë "to give", nëë "to take", and choo "to come".[470] These verbs take divergent plural suffixes from other verbs. In Western Alemannic, they take /-n/ in the western Einheitsplural-area, while the eastern area has /-nt/, e.g. Low Alemannic hen vs. Swabian hent '(we/you/they) have'. Non-short verbs show other plural endings, such as -ed (e.g. mached).[414] The contracted verbs are typically uncontracted in their "present subjunctive" forms; the three most frequent verbs, "to do", "to be" and "to have" have added an inorganic final -g that helps differentiate them from their indicative forms: Zurich German tüeg/syyg/hääg.[471]
In Swiss Alemannic short verbs take umlaut in the plural, similar to model verbs (e.g. gönd "we/you/they go").[472] Umlauted plural forms are also found in most Bavarian dialects for tun, gehn, and stehn. The verbs have additionally received a thematic plural ending, e.g. Upper Bavarian tenɐ "(we/they) do".[473]
In modern standard Dutch, there are six "short verbs," zijn "to be", doen "to do", gaan "to go", staan "to stand", as well as zien "to see" and slaan "to hit", the latter two the result of contraction; like in German, only "to be" preserves final -n (ik ben "I am"),[474] with the West Flemish of Westhoek even preserving final -m (ik bem). In dialects, these verbs (and sometimes other contracted verbs) have preserved or acquired a number of morphological peculiarities, including sometimes a first person singular in -n.[413] In the North Hollandic, forms such as ik doen are used generally, whereas in southern Hollandic the n is only inserted to avoid hiatus before the verb and a following vowel (doen ik and also ik doen et); in this same context, many dialects insert an -n to the short verbs zien (to see) and slaan (to hit) to avoid hiatus in inversion (zien ik). These forms may be found in near-dialect colloquial Dutch (tussentaal) more generally, even when not in hiatus. The same strategy of -n to avoid hiatus is found in southern Flemish and Brabantine, but in other areas -t, -d -j, or -g may be inserted.[475][380] Some dialects from the plural by adding an additional -e: we doene/gane/stane, probably on analogy with other dialects that have syncopated final -n in the thematic verbs and thus have plurals of two syllables (e.g. we leve "we live"); Zeelandic and Overflakkee instead add -e (schwa) directly to the root we doee, gae, stae.[476]
sîn ('to be')
[edit]gân/gên ('to go') and stân/stên ('to stand')
[edit]The vowel of the athematic verbs "to go" and "to stand" varies throughout the continental West Germanic dialect continuum, making it a useful marker of dialect.[an] Generally:[478][479][480]
- the form gân/stân is characteristic of all Low German and Low Franconian dialects,[476] along with parts of Ripuarian, parts of Moselle Franconian, southern Alemannic, and West Upper Alemannic except for Alsatian.
- the form gên/stên is characteristic of Ripuarian between the ik/ich and maken/machen lines, Moselle Franconian south of the op/auf line, Rhine Franconian, Alsatian, East Middle German, North Upper German, and East Upper German.
- transitional forms, with â for some forms and ê for others, can be found in the remainder of Moselle Franconian and Lower Alemannic in Baden.[481]
Some Modern Swabian dialects show a mixture of the two roots in the present: 1st person singular and imperative: (ʃdand, gaŋ, plural ʃdandəd gaŋgəd). Some of these forms can also be found in Low Alemannic in Baden and Alsace, although they have the gên/stên vowel. Additionally, a few Swabian dialects, mostly near the border between Bavarian and Alemannic, have ganga as the infinitive.[481][479]
In Central Franconian, South Low Franconian and also in many Low German dialects, the second and third person singular forms of gân/gên and stân/stên reflect earlier *gais/*stais and *gait/*stait (e.g. Cologne Ripuarian jeis/jeit, infinitive: jɔːn), which most likely go back to analogical formations with thematic verb endings (e.g. *gais < *gâ-is). Such forms are already attested in the Franconian variety of Old High German in Otfrid's Evangelienbuch.[482][483][484] In the Alemannic gân/stân area, Highest Alemannic dialects display the same forms in the second and third person singular (e.g. Visperterminen geisch/geit),[485] while other Swiss Alemannic dialects have invariant *â here (e.g. Zurich (High Alemannic) gaasch/gaat), Basel (Low Alemannic) goosch/goot).[486]
All continental West Germanic dialects with a surviving preterite use the longer forms to form the preterite. For gân, this form (e.g. Dutch/German ging) replaced an earlier suppletive preterite form still found in Old English. The preterite of stân originally lacked the nasal, as in Old High German stōd and Brabantine Middle Dutch stoet. These were replaced with forms including the n (German stand, Dutch stond).[477] The stem used in the past participle varies by dialect. In earlier continental West Germanic, two forms were available: athematic ge-gân, ge-stân and the longer ge-gangen ge-standen. In Low German, most dialects use the the short athematic stems to form the past participle: this was already the case in Middle Low German.[481] Dutch dialects show the same development, although the longer form is also attested in Middle Dutch.[487] German Low Franconian, agrees with neighboring Ripuarian and most High German dialects in having the longer forms (Ripuarian jəʃtanə, jəjaˑŋə); some High Alemannic dialects also have gųː from earlier ge-gân[488]
dôn ('to do')
[edit]In analogy to gân/gên and stân/stên, Central Franconian, South Low Franconian and many Low German dialects have developed second and third person singular forms of the type *dais/*dait (e.g. Cologne Ripuarian deis/deit, infinitive: dun).[483][489] The border between deit and duːt runs south of the Moselle, parallel to difference between geit and gaːt. Westphalian Low German instead has umlaut. This umlauted form is also shared with East Central German, e.g. Thuringian düːst, düːt.[490]
Large parts of Thuringian and Upper Saxon feature an imperative form duk - this has probably been adopted on the basis of the similar sak/sā "say". The similar form dux is found in neighboring Low German Brandenburgish.[490] A similar form with final [x] is found in Dutch Low Saxon and eastern Dutch dialects.[413]
hân < habên ('to have')
[edit]The vowel of the verb "to have" varies between High German on the one hand and Low German and Low Franconian on the other. Low German and Low Franconian both generally have /e/, along with some High German dialects in the Rhineland and a few disconnected ones further south. This characteristic difference comes from the verb belonging to two different weak verb classes in earlier West Germanic. Whereas Low Franconian and Low Saxon shared a form hebbian, a class I weak verb ending in -jan, Old High German had habēn, a class II weak verb ending in -ēn.[ao] As a result, the Low Franconian and Saxon forms underwent umlaut and West Germanic gemination, whereas the High German forms did not.[491][492][493]
In Dutch and Low German dialects, the conjugation of hebben historically shows a change in consonants from /b/ to /v/ in the 2nd and 3rd person singular, along the lines of: ik hebbe, du hevest, hi hevet. In Low German, however, most dialects have leveled the first person singular to ik hevve or ik heff. The plural has only been leveled in Westphalian, Eastphalian, and East Pommeranian (hevvet/heft/het/hevve, and the infinitive has remained unaffected in most dialects.[422] Dutch dialects maintain the contrast, with earlier hevet becoming heeft via schwa syncope.[493] In Brabantine and Limburgish, the /f/ has been lost entirely, giving heet and variants; South Hollandish instead has heit.[463]
Contracted forms of the verb haben remain common in High German dialects.[494] Variants of ich han is found in Rhine and Central Franconian, Alemannic, and Upper Austrian and Tyrolean Bavarian. The uncontracted first-person singular (e.g. ich habe) is most common in East Franconian, in Palatine, in Lower Alemannic on the Rhine, and in much of Bavarian.[495] The second and third person singular are contracted in all dialects except some Bavarian ones, which have forms similar to habst and habt. Contracted forms in the plural are found in Alemannic and Central Franconian.[496]
Unlike High German, most Low Franconian and Low German dialects did not contract the verb "to have".[497] However, on the border with Central German, Low German plural forms hat and han, as well as a first person singular hä and infinitive hän in South Brandenburgish, show High German influence.[498] Contracted forms of "to have" are found in East Flemish and the southeastern West Flemish (ik hèn), in some isolated pockets in Zeelandic. This is probably a native contraction of a form *heb-n with a final -n for the first person singular.[499]
In the South Low Franconian dialect area, the divide between contracted hān from Old High German habên and uncontracted hebbe(n)/hevve(n) from Old Low Franconian/Old Saxon hebbian largely conincides with the sagen-line, which separates reflexes of Old High German sagên 'to say' from those of Old Low Franconian/Old Saxon seggian.[500][ap] The sagen-line marks the border between the transitional East Limburgish–Ripuarian dialect area and East Limburgish (and all other subgroups of South Low Franconian).[501][502] Contracted hān is thus predominantly found the German part of the South Low Franconian dialect area and in the southeasternmost part of Dutch Limburg and in eastern Belgia around Eupen, while uncontracted hebbe(n)/hevve(n) is predominantly found in Dutch and Belgian Limburg, and additionally in the northwest of the German part (see map).[499][503]
lân < lâzzan < lâtan ('to let')
[edit]The contracted form lân is only found in very conservative dialects today. It can still be found in some Alemmanic and Bavarian dialects, such as Swiss German laa (Zurich) or loo (Basel),[472] or in North Bavarian Upper Palatinate German forms like lout ("he lets").[504][494]
Modal and preterite-present verbs
[edit]Proto-West Germanic attests 12 verbs that inflect in the present as other verbs do in the preterite (preterite presents), descending from the Proto-Indo-European stative conjugation. Only 11 of these verbs are attested in continental West Germanic. Preterite present verbs share peculiarities of conjugation in the present tense, including: 1) the lack of an ending in the first and third person singular; 2) the use of a second person singular ending -t in the earliest attestations; 3) the existence of ablaut between the singular and plural in several verbs. Additionally, the verbs formed their preterite tense by adding a dental suffix without any intervening vowel, e.g. Old High German tohta.[505] In Old High German, the preterite present verbs appear as (listing first the singular then the plural): weiz/wizzum "know", *eih/eigum "have", toug/tugum "be useful", darf/durfum "need", gi-tar/gi-turrum "dare", kann/kunnum "know, understand", an/unnum "grant", scal/skulum "should, to owe", gi-nah/*gi-nuhum "to be enough", mag/magum "to be able", muoz/muozzum "be able, have opportunity".[506][507]
Originally, all of these verbs had full lexical meaning. However, over time, many of them become model verbs while others cease to preterite present verbs; other verbs are added to the model category as well.[508] Modal verbs, as they have been added to the category, have taken on aspects of the special morphology of the other preterite-present verbs. This begins in the early Middle Ages with the verb wellen "to want", originally an optative athematic verb.[509][510]
A number of preterite present verbs have either been lost entirely or moved to either the strong or weak conjugations, particularly those without modal use. The verb ginah is already extinct in early Old High German; only attested twice.[511][512] Modern Dutch, modern High German, and modern Low German have all lost the verb *eih/eigum.[513][514] In Old High German, it is only attested in the plural and the present subjunctive; other forms use habēn, the ancestor of the modern German verb haben "to have".[515] This usage had already become rare in the High Middle Ages.[516] German gönnen/Dutch gunnen, a form of earlier an/unnum with a syncopated prefix ge-, has become a normal weak verb in all Dutch and High and Low German dialects; German taugen/Dutch deugen has become a regular, non-model verb in all Dutch and High German dialects, but in Low German it has remained a preterite present verb in parts of Brandenburgish and Low Prussian.[517][518][519]
Conjugation
[edit]Preterite-present verbs generally take no ending in the first and third person singular. Non-apocopating Dutch dialects such as that of Goeree may add an -e to the first person singular, as for other verbs: ik wete "I know".[514] West Flemish dialects sometimes show forms with final -n, e.g. ik moetn/moen rather than ik moet.[519] In High and Low German dialects, on the other hand, the tendency is to give modal verbs regular 3rd person singular endings, e.g. Low German dörft, North Bavarian diärf(t), khōd. In Alemannic, wissen regularly takes a -t in the third person singular, e.g. Swabian wɔest. This final change was also found in standard German in the 18th century.[520]
Most dialects have replaced the original preterite-present 2nd person singular ending -t with -s(t), as on other verbs. The most common exception is wit (from older willt). It is found in many dialects from Hessian to the southwest, and can also be found in some Central German dialects as well as Low German. Most other modal verbs have taken on the standard ending. However, relicts of the original -t ending remain e.g. Thuringian du kant, eastern Hessian kand.[521] Dutch Low Saxon has also retained the ending -t in some places, such as Groningen: doe zelt "you shall". In Dutch, the change from the older pronoun du to gij brought with it a -t ending from the plural; however, before this, there is some evidence for a shift from older -t forms to -s(t) forms (e.g. du macht as well as du maechs).[522]
Originally, most preterite present verbs showed ablaut between the singular and plural in the present tense. Additionally, in High and Low German, the plural ablaut of the preterite-present verbs have frequently shown umlaut since the High Middle Ages, e.g. ich kann and wir können (or, with unrounding, kennen); Low German ik will vs. we wüllt.[aq] From the plural, it gradually spread into the infinitive as well. The origin of this umlaut is debated. Theories include:[525]
- the umlaut was formed due to enclitic pronouns containing the vowel i, causing umlaut (e.g. durfen wir > dürfen wir);
- it originated in via analogy to the weak verbs with "Rückumlaut" (e.g. Middle High German present plural grüezen, preterite gruozte influenced müezen, muozte
- it originated in the subjunctive forms, which regularly showed umlaut, including in the "present subjunctive" (e.g. ich müsse), although this form does not produce umlaut in any other verbs;
- it comes from a desire to more clearly differentiate the plural from the singular, similar to the use of Umlaut in High and Low German nouns.
Some dialects have lost the ablaut between singular and plural, e.g. some Bavarian and Alemmanic dialects ich derf and wir derfen, or dialectal Dutch ik kann wij kannen.[526] Among Low and High German dialects, in Low German, most dialects maintain separate stems for müssen, sollen, and dürfen, but not for wollen. In Central German, most dialects maintain separate stems for müssen and wollen, but only some for dürfen and very few for sollen. Upper German dialects reflect the same pattern as Central German.[527] sollen is the form without umlaut in the plural with the widest distribution in modern dialects.[528] Only Low German and southwestern Upper German dialects regularly show a difference between the singular and plural stems of sollen.[529]
Contracted forms of modal verbs (that is, lacking their stem final consonant) are common in German dialects. In High and Low German dialects, the modal verbs often appear in contracted forms that lack their final consonant. The most common simplification is the loss of l in sollen/wollen before the 2nd person singular ending -s(t) and the preterite ending -t. The l is lost in almost all Low German dialects, as well as in southern Hessian, Palatine, Lorraine Franconian, South Franconian, and Alemmanic. Contracted plural forms are also found in southern Swabian and Alemmanic and Swiss Alemannic. Contracted plural forms are also found in Thuringian. Contracted forms of müssen in the plural are found in Alemannic Alsatian, southern Swabian, and eastern Swiss German, only the last of which also has short forms in the singular, as well as Bavarian dialects and Thuringian. The final g in mag is lost in several Upper and Central German dialects, but it has sometimes been restored by analogy.[530] In Swiss Alemannic, all five modal verbs occur in contracted forms (e.g. chöü, möü, söü, wei, mue). können has short forms throughout Alemannic in the singular due to final nasal deletion, but in the plural only in western Swiss Alemannic. In all Alemannic areas with contracted forms, the contracted modal verbs take the same ending as the so-called "short-verbs".[531][532][533]
*darf/*durf and *dar/*durr
[edit]The oldest attested forms of German dürfen, Dutch durven are:
Dialect | present singular | present plural | preterite | participle |
---|---|---|---|---|
Old High German | darf | durfun | dorfta | - |
Old Saxon | tharf | thurƀun | thorfta | - |
Middle Dutch | darf | dorven | dorfte/dorste | gedorfd |
The verb's form has been simplified or altered in many dialects. Many dialects that otherwise do not lose syllable final /r/ have lost it here, e.g. in Swabian.[534] Low German dialects instead often show metathesis of the r, giving forms like West Low German draf.[535] Most High German dialects have umlaut in both the singular and the plural, a process probably aided by some phonetic changes in these dialects: Thuringian derf/derfən; the form with a in the singular is still found in most Low German, Ripuarian, and Swiss Alemannic dialects.[536]
High German, Dutch, and Low German have all confused *darf/*durf (originally "to need") with *dar/*durr "to dare". In High German this secondary meaning of the verb has sense died out,[537] but confusion with *dar/*durr remained. For instance, Alemannic dialects continue to show forms lacking a final f or mixing the paradigms (e.g. East High Alemannic daa "is allowed" or Swabian dɛf (<darf) and dɛrʃt (<darst).[538] Likewise, in parts of North Low German and Pommeranian Low German, dörren (from *dar/*dur) has replaced dröven, taking on its meaning?[535] At least one Westphalian dialect has preserved the original meaning "dare" (he dɛstə "he dared") and kept it separate from the verb "to be allowed to".[521] In Dutch, "to dare" has become the primary meaning of durven. The archaic preterite dorst comes from the second root. However, in modern Dutch, the verb has become a a regular weak verb in most dialects. It has developed a strong past tense (dierf) in southern dialects. In South Brabantine, *dar/*dur has survived as tèère "to dare". The form duurn has survived in Dutch Low Saxon in the meaning "to dare".[539]
In Old High German *darf/*durf is used almost exclusively with a negation to show that something is not necessary. This meaning survives in some modern dialects. Later, the negation developed the meaning "not to be able to". In the 15th century, it comes to be used with this meaning without negation, with the meaning "to be allowed."[540]
*kan/*kun
[edit]In the earliest attested Germanic languages, *kan/*kun was mostly used to show intellectual ability. It began to show ability more generally during the high medieval period. In High German, it did not fully replace the verb mögen in the meaning "to be able" until the 18th century.[541][523]
*mag
[edit]The oldest attested forms of *mag are:
Dialect | present singular | present plural | preterite | participle |
---|---|---|---|---|
Old High German | mag | magun/mugun | mahta/mohta | - |
Old Saxon | mag/mah | mugun | mahta/mohta | - |
Middle Dutch | mag | mogen | mochte | ghemoghen |
In Old High German, *mag meant something like "to be able to". After the 16th century, it increasingly came to mean "to like" in High German, being replaced in the meaning "to be able to" by können by the 18th century.[542] In Dutch, the meaning has instead developed to "be allowed to" (cf. English "may").[523]
*mōt
[edit]Dialect | present singular | present plural | preterite | participle |
---|---|---|---|---|
Old High German | muoz | muozun | muosa | - |
Old Saxon | mōt | mōtun | mōsta | - |
Middle Dutch | moet | moeten | moeste | ghemoeten |
*mōt originally showed the same form for both the singular and plural. As it still does in Dutch and Low German dialects.[citation needed] In High German, a distinction developed between the singular and plural by inserting umlaut (muoz/müezen—the earliest modal verb for which the use of umlaut in the plural can be shown.[543] In central German, the formerly long diphthong of muoz and müezen came to be pronounced short during the late Middle Ages. In Upper German, it remains a long diphthong (e.g. Alemannic mer müend "we must").[544]
In southern Westphalian, maut (< Middle Low German môt 'must') lacks its own preterite form, instead using the preterite of mag (moxtə).[518]
In the earliest attested stages of the Germanic languages, muozan was mostly used to mean "to enable", contrasting with the verbs kunnan and mugan in that it referred to external circumstances rather than internal ability. In the 10th century, it developed the meaning "to force" in High German, but was also used to express wishes in both German and Dutch. It developed the present meaning in all varieties of "to need to" "to be required to" in the 1200s.[545][523][546]
*skal/*skul
[edit]The oldest attested forms of *skal/*skul are:
Dialect | present singular | present plural | preterite | participle |
---|---|---|---|---|
Old High German | scal | sculun | scolta | - |
Old Saxon | skal | skulun | skolda | - |
Middle Dutch | sal | sullen | soude | - |
In most German and Dutch dialects, the initial /sk/ of the Proto-West Germanic verb *skal/*skul- 'shall' has been simplified to /s/, possibly due to the word's frequent occurrence in enclitic positions.[547] This form developed first in Old Low Franconian in the 9th century and spread from there to High German, where it replaced earlier initial /ʃ/.[548] Some Bavarian dialects such as Upper Palatinate have maintained initial /ʃ/ (e.g. schol) to the present day. While older stages of Low German almost exclusively showed an intial /sk/, /sx/, or /ʃ/ for the verb schölen, in the modern dialects this form has receded northward under High German influence. It is now limited to North Low German, except Hamburg and East Frisian Low German, and Eastern Pomeranian.[549]
Among High German dialects, the change from schal to sol was often accompanied by the shift of a to o.[548] All Dutch and most Low German dialects continue to show a in the singular. The plural originally showed u, but changed to o in many High German dialects. Upper German and Low German then frequently umlauted this vowel (söllen). Other vowels appear in various dialects.[532]
In the earliest attested stages of the Germanic languages, *skal/*skul showed obligation or necessity. In this last meaning, it comes to be replaced by müezen over the high medieval period.[550][523] In Dutch and Low German?[citation needed], it comes to be used primarily as an auxiliary to form the future tense.[551]
*well
[edit]The verb *well was not originally a preterite present verb, but rather an optative verb. However, it developed the traits of other preterite present verbs already in the early Middle Ages.[509] In earlier High German, the stem tended to alternate between will and wel(len). In East Central German, the plural /e/ has become /o/, a change which has expanded into many West Central and also Upper German dialects. Among modern dialects, Alemannic and Bavarian dialects often retain the old /e/ vowel in the plural (e.g. Upper Palatinate Bavarian mer welle "we want"), sometimes rounding it to /ö/. Low German instead shows /i/ in both singular and plural (with the plural sometime rounded to /ü/, depending on dialect), as do most Low Franconian (Dutch) dialects; Central Franconian originally has /i/ (sometimes lowered to /e/) but has often adopted /o/ from East Central German.[552]
The verb *well is attested meaning "to want" since Old High German.[550]
*wait/*wit
[edit]New modal verbs
[edit]Complementizer agreement
[edit]A feature known as "complementizer agreement" is common to most continental West Germanic dialects, as well as Frisian, but is absent from standard German and Dutch. In this feature, endings are added to complementizers such as conjunctions, but also to question words and relative pronouns, so that they agree with the subject of the clause. It is found throughout a wide-range of varieties and shows a great deal of homogeneity where it is found.[553][554][555]
Most dialects with the feature belong to one of two areal groups:
- Most Frisian, Eastern Dutch dialects, and German dialects typically show agreement with the 2nd person singular, e.g. Westphalian vanste "if/when you", with the -ste agreeing with the 2nd person singular subject. This is the form of complementizer agreement with the greatest areal distribution. Dialects featuring this system include Middle Franconian, Rhine Franconian, as well as some variants of Alemannic and southern Bavarian.[556][557]
- Most Western Dutch dialects instead show agreement for number, e.g. ase we hore (Katwijk Dutch) "when we hear", with -e marking the plural subject.[557]
Some West Flemish dialects show agreement for all persons, although this is in some case via a zero-suffix. Other dialects, mainly clustered in adjoining regions of the East Central and northern Upper German groups, such as Upper Saxon, Thuringian, East Franconian, and North Bavarian, show agreement for all persons in the plural as well as the second person singular, e.g. North Bavarian dasn mer "that we", with the -n showing first person plural agreement.[558][557]
This feature is first attested in 13th century Dutch, but it is not attested in German at all until far more recently.[554] Some dialects lack the feature, such as the Dutch of Utrecht, North and South Brabant, Gelderland, and Antwerp; the German areal distribution requires more research.[559]
Nouns
[edit]Gender system
[edit]Standard Dutch and northern Dutch dialects have merged the masculine and feminine genders into a single common gender; southern dialects, however, maintain the three gender system. The merger of masculine and feminine is also found in the Low German spoken in East Frisia.[560][561]
Case system
[edit]The earliest continental West Germanic languages attest four cases robustly: nominative, accusative, dative, and genitive. A fifth case, the instrumental, was retained on only some nouns stems and pronominal forms.[562] Standard German retains a four-case system. However, the number of contexts in which the genitive case is used in the standard language has steadily decreased.[563] Most dialects have eliminated the genitive case and simplified the gender system in other ways, although a few have maintained a three case system of nominative, accusative, dative.[564] Standard Dutch and its dialects have lost case distinctions except for a few relic expressions.[565] In all dialects, case marking is now primarily found on determiners and modifiers rather than on the noun.[566]
Most Low German dialects have simplified the case system to nominative vs. a single oblique case by merging the dative and accusative, an innovation visible already before 1500. Distinct dative morphology, namely the addition of a final schwa singular nouns of all three genders, is still found in (moribund) Eastern Low German dialects as well as Eastphalian and Westphalian.[567] Western High German dialects have either retained a three case system or else merged the nominative and accusative while keeping a distinct dative case. Western dialects maintaining all three cases tend to be more centrally located, while those merging are found more on the periphery. Eastern dialects instead either have a nominative-oblique distinction, as in Low German, or else have merged all three cases.[568][569]
The only Low Franconian dialect to maintain any sort of case inflection is found in Vorden, part of Lower Geldric; it maintains a difference between the nominative and dative/accusative for adjectives, and may add a final dative -e to masculine/neuter nouns.[570] Otherwise, Dutch dialects had lost an independent dative case by the mid 19th century and the genitive by the end of that century[571] The nominative and the accusative were also merged. In northern dialects, the nominative case became the default form, resulting in a merger of the masculine and feminine genders. In southern dialects, the accusative case became the default instead.[561]
Pluralization
[edit]High and Low German has maintained a very complex plural system, with several different potential endings. Standard Dutch, in contrast, has simplified to just two endings, -en and -s.[16][572] Additionally, German has steadily developed a system whereby the plural form is closely associated with gender. In standard German, 90% of inanimate masculine nouns take the ending -e, 70% of feminine nouns take the ending -(e)n, 70% of neuter nouns take the ending -e and 20% take the ending -er.[573] Dutch, however, shows no such correlation between gender and plurality, a difference that had already developed in the 12th century.[574] Different dialects handle pluralization differently.
-e plurals
[edit]The plural ending -e (pronounced as schwa [ə]) developed from two sources: a-stem masculine nouns (e.g. Old High German tag - tagā "day - days") and i-stem nouns of all three genders (e.g. Old High German gast - gesti "guest - guests"). The i-stem category was productive way of pluralizing in Old Low Franconian and Old High German, but more rare in Ingvaeonic dialects such as Old Saxon. In Old Low Franconian and Old High German, the class was expanded mostly with feminine nouns, such as craft "strength".[575]
i-stem plurals developed umlaut in the plural, as seen in Old High German/Old Saxon gesti "guests". In Middle High and Middle Low German and even more so in Early New High German, many formerly a-stem nouns come to be pluralized with umlaut.[576][577][578] In modern Low German, however, plurals with umlaut are less common than in High German dialects.[579] In coastal Dutch dialects, where only primary umlaut of /ǎ/ took place, most of these umlauted plural forms were removed by the Middle Dutch period, giving e.g. Middle Dutch gaste. Exceptions, such as stad - steden, may show High German influence.[580] Umlauted plural forms are found in Southeastern Low Franconian, which experienced more umlauting phenomena.[581]
In the Middle High German period, the ending -e was extended beyond former a-stem masculine nouns and i-stem nouns to also encompass many neuter a-stem nouns, which formerly took no ending.[582] Modern High German dialects without schwa-apocope frequently pluralize masculine nouns by adding final -e, and in some dialects with schwa-apocope, the plural ending -e has been restored.[583] It is either restored in all instances or if it would otherwise be impossible to distinguish the singular from the plural by other means, such as umlaut.[584][240] East Central German dialects have frequently added the ending to two-syllable nouns that would otherwise have no ending: Upper Saxon epelə "apples".[585]
In Old Saxon, a-stem nouns typically take a plural ending -as/-os rather than -a (see s-plural); the -a ending is first attested in southwestern dialects along the Rhine in the 9th century. Arjen Versloot argues for Low Franconian influence as the cause; other scholars suggest High German influence.[586] In Middle Low German, the use of -e (from a-stems and i-stems) was greatly extended, encompassing many old masculine a-stems that formerly took -os as well as neuter a-stems that took no ending.[587] In modern Low German dialects, the ending -e is only preserved in southwestern dialects, with others having experienced schwa apocope.[579]
In Dutch, the inherited plural ending -e was lost during the late Middle Ages, being replaced with the endings -en or -s.[588] Many High and Low German varieties with apocope have likewise either moved these words to the zero-change plural category or else innovated new pluralization strategies for words that formerly took -e plural.[589]
-er plurals
[edit]The modern plural ending -er originates with the Proto-Germanic s- stem neuter nouns. This class of nouns was very small in the earliest West Germanic languages, mostly limited to young animals.[590] The earliest form of this ending has an important isogloss for West Germanic languages: in most Old High German it is -ir, causing umlaut (lemmir "lambs", whereas in other West Germanic languages, including the Central Franconian dialects of Old High German, the same stem appears as -ere, (lammere) without umlaut.[591]
In High German, the use of -er plurals has steadily grown to encompass ever more neuter nouns over time, especially in the early modern period.[592] Western Central and Upper German dialects and Bavarian have often extended its use onto nouns that do not take -er plural in other dialects (e.g. better "beds"). Many dialects also use -er to form the plural of the diminutive -chen: South Hessian fēgəlxər "little birds". East Central German uses the ending less frequently than other dialects.[585] In most High German dialects, the -er plural is always accompanied by umlaut if possible.[593] However, in Central Franconian, especially Ripuarian, umlautless forms of some -er plural nouns remain to the present.[594] Both standard German and dialects have applied -er to some masculine nouns, but these were all formerly primarily or only used in the neuter.[585][595]
Low German also extended the use of -er nouns in the early modern period, but in most dialects it is not as frequent as in High German.[596] In the area around the Weser, it has also spread to many masculine nouns (e.g. stēner "stones"), but in the remaining Low German dialects it is almost exclusively restricted to neuter nouns. West Low German -er plurals do not show umlaut (e.g. kalver "calves", lammer "lambs"), but East Low German has generally adopted umlauted forms from High German influence.[597] In Westphalian, the ending remains the unsyncopated /-ərə/.[596] Dutch Low Saxon shows the same expansion, sometimes with umlaut, e.g. holt - hölter "wood - woods".[598]
Dutch did not extend the use of -er as far as High or Low German and the ending is no longer productive. In Standard Dutch, 15 neuter nouns still take a plural form -eren, formed from -er + the still productive plural form -en.[588] This plural developed from the earlier -ere in Middle Dutch. In dialects, the -eren form is found in Brabantine and dialects further to the east. In southwestern dialects, the plural -s was added instead, giving -ers. Some eastern dialects have the simple ending -er.[599] Southeastern Low Franconian (Limburgish) shows a greater number of -er plurals, including some otherwise found in High German dialects: daːkər "rooves", huːzər "houses". Some Limburgish -er plurals feature umlaut, as in High and Low German, but there is a tendency for this umlaut to be replaced with the unmutated vowel: lamər or lemər "lambs".[600]
-en plurals
[edit]The -en plural ending originates with the class of weak nouns, which could belong to the masculine, feminine, or neuter genders. During the high and late Middle Ages, the ending was also extended to feminine o-stem nouns, which had come to end in schwa in both the singular and plural.[citation needed]
In German, -en is the normal pluralization strategy for feminine nouns and "weak nouns". Many Central and High German dialects apocopate final -n, so that for those dialects that do not apocopate final -e, a difference between singular and plural is lost in these classes. Sometimes, these dialects innovate a change in vowel quality to retain a singular - plural distinction.[601] Besides feminine and weak nouns, Low German has moved nouns that formerly used -e in the plural to the -en pluralization.[602]
The spread of the -en plural in Dutch to nouns that formerly took -e is probably on analogy with verb verbs; additionally, Middle Dutch nouns with an -e plural became -en in the dative, which may have aided the spread of the -en plural in other instances.[603] Many feminine nouns likewise acquired an -en plural via a merger with the weak declension.[604]
-s plurals
[edit]Like some of instances of the ending -e, the ending -s originates with the a-stem masculine nouns. It is sometimes considered a shared North Sea Germanic feature. Its precise origins are uncertain; it appears to continue the old nominative plural form -ōz, possibly with secondary stress.[605][606] In English it is the almost exclusive ending, whereas it is not native to High German. For Germanic a-stems, Old English has -ās as the sole pluralization strategy, whereas Old Saxon attests both -os/-as and -a.[607]
Old Saxon appears to have originally only had a plural form -os, but adopted the alternative ending -a under either High German or Low Franconian influence.[586] Although it became rare in the Middle Low German period, the ending -s survived and becomes more common from the mid 14th century onward, when it begins to be used with words denoting people, especially those ending in -er, then also -el and words denoting familial relationship.[608] In modern Low German dialects, -s is the most productive plural ending. It has become the ending for any word that does not pluralize in another way, including those who have lost a final -e plural via schwa apocope. It is also used with nouns ending in -er, -el, -en, as well as sometimes pleonastically with the plural ending -en (giving a double ending -ens). For the diminutive suffix -ken, a plural form is used in which the final -n is lost: Westphalian snäpskəs[609][596]
The origins of the s-plural in Dutch dialects are disputed; these become common in the Middle Dutch period.[610] In minor Old Low Franconian sources, -as plurals such as nestas ("nests") are attested, but it is possible that these derive from Frisian, Anglo-Saxon, or even Latin influence on the manuscripts: the main sources in Old Low Franconian only attest -a plurals.[611][612] It has variously been argued that the frequent -s ending in Middle Dutch originated in language contact with Old French, speading from the area of Calais into other dialects; that the form originated in coastal dialects and is a native Ingvaeonic feature; or that it originated in eastern dialects under the influence of Old Saxon.[613] Among modern dialects, It is most common and productive in Flemish, and is also common in Northeastern dialects, where it is commonly used for monosyllabic nouns and nouns ending in -er.[614] In Hollandish, -s is added to nouns ending in schwa, which receive -n in Standard Dutch, and in the form -es to those ending in a consonant (e.g. kip - kippes "chicken - chickens"). In West Frisian, the variant -se is found.[615] Limburgish has an -s plural much like standard Dutch (e.g. vaders "fathers"), but it sometimes combines it with umlaut: broːr - brøːrs "brother - "brothers", zoːn - zøːns "son - sons". The is probably a recent development in Limburgish, coming from Western dialects or standard Dutch, explaining how it came to be combined with umlaut.[600]
In Old and Middle High German, no plural forms in -s are attested, but -s plurals have become common in High German since the Early New High German period through mostly the influence of Low German and French: the earliest attestations are on loanwords from these languages.[616][609] The ending is very rare in Upper German dialects, but not uncommon in Central German dialects, especially East Central German. It is frequently used for words that do not have a distinct plural, such as die Kumpels "the buddies" (rather than prescribed die Kumpel).[617][601]
Zero-ending plurals
[edit]In early West Germanic, neuter a-stem nouns had a plural form that lacked any ending, caused by apocope of the final unstressed vowel -u: Old High German wort (singular) and wort (plural).[618] Generally, these unmarked forms were given new plural endings during the Middle Ages, adding -e or -er.[582] Later Dutch has replaced -e with -en. However, endingless plurals still survive in some Dutch dialects (e.g. schaap "sheep" [pl]).[619] Some High German dialects, primarily Swiss Alemannic, have preserved the endingless neuter plural more generally: xind "child - children" ek "corner - corners".[579]
Plurals without any ending are required in standard German for most nouns ending in -en, -er, and -el in the singular. These plurals developed in the transition from old to middle stages of continental West German via a syncope of the final ending -e following an unstressed syllable (Old High German nagala > Middle High German nagel "nails").[620] Some of these have since become umlauted in standard German (Middle High German nagel > modern German Nägel "nails), but many show no change.[621] Middle Dutch vacillated between adding -en or -s in these cases; modern Dutch usually adds -s.[610][622] Most High German dialects have likewise applied new pluralization strategies such as umlaut or a new ending to these nouns.[601] Plurals without any marking at all are likewise virtually nonexistent in Low German.[596]
Central and Upper German dialects which experienced schwa-apocope often have zero-ending plurals. Plurality is instead frequently marked by umlaut, which is now the most frequent pluralization strategy in Upper German.[623] In the former i-stems, this umlaut is inherited, but in many words it is motivated only by morphology: Tag (s.) Täg (cf. standard Tage). Other dialects may use variations in vowel length and consonant quality to mark plurality.[624][592] These same options are also found in Low German dialects with schwa syncope: (e.g. Brandeburgisch arm - ärm "arm - arms", Brandenburgish dax då~g "day - days").[625]
Some High German dialects featuring both syncope of final -e and final -en feature large classes of nouns without any plural marking.[601]
Diminutive formation
[edit]The most common form of the diminutive suffix is divided between the north and the south of the West Germanic dialect continuum, between suffixes derived from an earlier -lîn (southern) or an earlier -kîn (northern). This contrast dates back to the common Germanic period.[626][361]
Upper German dialects, along with southern East Central German, generally show a suffix contain an -l (e.g. standard German -lein, Swabian -le, Bavarian -(e)l).[627] Northern dialects instead form their diminutive with a velar or palatal consonant. The standard Dutch diminutive -tje has come from a palatalization of an earlier -kijn, whereas the standard German -chen derives from central German dialects in which -ken underwent the High German consonant shift. Many Low German dialects have -ke(n), but Low German dialects around Hamburg have lost their diminutive suffix, whereas some dialects have developed new suffixes.[626][628]
Historical records indicate that Central German originally used an -l based suffix and adopted a -k based suffix from further north after the Old High German period.[629] In modern standard German, it is no longer possible to determine whether a speaker/writer is from the north or south based on diminutive use, as certain rules now determine which diminutive is used with which words.[630] Moreover, over the course of the Early New High German period, there is a marked shift for writers in all areas to use the -chen suffix as the default in most situations in the standard language.[631]
Literary languages
[edit]At the earliest stage of attestation in the Early Middle Ages, continental West Germanic dialects are documented in manifold regional scribal varieties, mostly linked to monasteries. Based on their broad lingustic properties, they can be assigned to three major groups (Old High German, Old Saxon and Old Dutch) that correspond to the three contemporary dialect groups.[632]. The coexistence of locally competing regional literary traditions continued into the Late Middle Ages, but for each of the three groups, supra-regional standardized varieties started to emerge. In the Dutch and High German areas, these were mostly linked to the production of literary works, while in the Low Saxon area, the Middle Low German standard language that florished from the 14th to the 16th century was tied to the influence of the Hanseatic League.[632][406] With the decline of the Hanseatic League and the emergence of the High German-based modern standard German language, the latter has become the literary language not only for the High German dialects, but also for much of the Low Saxon dialect area.[633] For Low Franconian, the transition from Middle Dutch to Modern Dutch went along with a regional shift from Branbantian-based standard Middle Dutch to Hollandic-based standard Modern Dutch.[634]
Until the 20th century, standard Dutch and standard German were the only standardized literary languages in the continental West Germanic continuum. In the late 20th century, Luxembourgish emerged as a literary standard based on the local Moselle Franconian variety of Luxembourg. In the Netherlands, Limburgish (i.e. the Low Franconian dialects of Dutch Limburg that mostly belong to the South Low Franconian group) gained offcial recognition as a regional language.[635]
Notes
[edit]- ^ The unshifted West Germanic varieties north of High German are sometimes collectively called "Low German" in a broad sense in traditional German dialectology. Since it is only defined by the lack of a sound shift that also did not occur in all other Germanic languages, it cannot serve as a criterion for classification.[7]
- ^ The form Gans is found in the East Low German dialects.[50]
- ^ There is disagreement about whether /g/ (/ɣ/) was originally a stop or a fricative; while most scholars argue that it was originally a fricative and progressively became a stop, Theodor Frings argued that it was originally a stop and progressively spirantized in different dialects.[57]
- ^ Some scholars have argued that /β ɣ/ remained fricatives word internally, on the basis of their status as fricatives in some German dialects. However, this is probably a later development, given the fact that these dialects devoice final /b/ and /g/ to /p/ and /k/.[53]
- ^ There is some debate whether the processes in Dutch and German are in fact different.[63]
- ^ Forms cited to Taeldeman 2013b, p. 160.
- ^ A northern outlier of the shift /st/ > /ʃt/ in non-initial position is found in the dialect of Wenden (Sauerland).[95]
- ^ Many dialects that experienced inner-German lenition also spirantized the lenis stops /b, d, g/ word-internally, thus maintaining a distinction. See Development of Proto-Germanic /β ð ɣ/.[81]
- ^ The older dialect of Ghent has a variant pronunciation /ŋɣ/ for final -ng, likely a step on the way to complete loss of the /g/.[148]
- ^ "Secondary" and "general umlaut" may be counted as a single process called "secondary umlaut".[156]
- ^ Due to the fronting of all instances of /*u/ to /y/ in Dutch, it is impossible to tell whether umlaut affected West Germanic /u/.[164]
- ^ In some early manuscripts, such as the Straubing fragment of the Heliand, this sound is spelled as ⟨a⟩ instead of common ⟨o⟩. This appears to be an Ingvaeonism that mostly appears in sources associated with Eastphalia, and is absent from later texts.[176]
- ^ The High German split of rising diphthongs is also found in the dialect of Wenden (south of Olpe) that is spoken in a small pocket sandwiched between the Low Saxon and Central German dialect areas, and which has not – like the South Low Franconian dialects – taken part in the High German consonant shift.[188][9]
- ^ In Ripuarian, /eː/ and /oː/ from ei and ou are also distinct from /eː/ and /oː/ that developed from OHG ie and uo, because they have different tone contours.
- ^ It is debated whether /eː/ and /oː/ are the result of secondary monophthongization of /ie/ and /uo/, or direct continuations of *ē² and *ō.
- ^ Traditionally called "toppled" diphthongs (gestürzte Diphthonge), because of the apparent reversal of the components of the diphthongs /ie/ → /ei/, /uo/ → /ou/.
- ^ These long high vowels from ē⁴ and ō¹ are never shortened and thus remain distinct from reflexes of the historical long high vowels *ī and *ū, which are shortened to /i/ and /u/ in most environments, e.g. bē⁴den > /biːdn̩/ 'offer' vs. bīten > /bitn̩/ 'bite'.
- ^ This change has had affects on morphology, as many grammatically significant endings had final schwa. In most instances, schwa syncope has not entered standard German, but it has entered standard Dutch. This leads to the difference between standard Dutch ik neem and standard German ich nehme (both: "I take").[241]
- ^ Epenthesis can be found in Old High German, where it was characteristic of Upper German, e.g. starah "strong". However, these examples are limited to early texts and show no continuity with the later phenomenon.[252]
- ^ Unrounding also did not occur in a few isolated areas such as Cimbrian, some Highest Alemannic varieties spoken by Walser colonies, and in a small contiguous pocket of northern Hessian and northern Thuringian dialects spoken close to the High German–Low German dialect boundary.[255]
- ^ The Low Prussian dialect still survives as Plautdietsch spoken by the Mennonite diaspora in the Americas.[258]
- ^ Evidence from unrounded relic forms in neighboring non-unrounding areas indicates that unrounding must have been more widespread in Belgian Brabant and probably even once formed a single area with Belgian Limburg. The rounded pronunciation was reintroduced for sociolinguistic reasons, having spread from the prestige dialects spoken in Brussels and Antwerp.[261][262]
- ^ In dialects that have undergone both unrounding and sponaneous fronting, morphological umlaut remains intact, although it is not realized as a back/front-alternation, but rather as a rounded/unrounded-alternation, as in Low Alemannic (Colmar dialect): /lyːs/ 'louse' ~ /liːs/ 'lice' (< /luːs/ 'louse' ~ /lyːsə/), /myətʀ̩/ 'mother' ~ /miətʀ̩/ 'mothers' (< /muətər/ ~ /myətər/).[278]
- ^ It is also possible that it began in different times in different regions, as there is some early evidence from Bavarian.[281]
- ^ There is debate about whether the lengthening of monosyllabic nouns ending in consonants in German is through analogy or a corresponding phonological process, "monosyllabic lengthening."[291] For instance, many High Alemannic dialects lacking open syllable lengthening experience "High Alemannic lengthening," in which closed monosyllabic short vowels are lengthened.[292]
- ^ Examples from standard German include hămer > Hămmer, kŏmen > kŏmmen. However, there are also many exceptions, e.g. vǎter > Vaːter.[295]
- ^ In Upper German dialects that retain geminates and have not undergone open syllable lengthening, these same environments produced geminate consonants.[298]
- ^ Standard Dutch retains the lowering of /i/ in met "with" (cf. German mit) and hem (cf. English him).[310]
- ^ This process shows exceptions such as zwart "black" and [undefined] Error: [undefined] Error: {{Lang}}: no text (help): no text (help) "hard", however, in Middle Dutch lengthened forms are also attested in at least some cases.[319]
- ^ In Flemish, this can also affect er that resulted from original ir, as in kark.[320]
- ^ The earlier Dutch realization of older West Germanic *au merged into this sound in the 19th century. However, the two earlier sounds are still spelled differently, with older *au (Middle Dutch /ou/) being spelled ⟨au⟩, e.g. blauw "blue".[328]
- ^ Similarly, /l/ velarization in East Central German is usually connected to Slavic influence.[327]
- ^ Middle Dutch attested a form unsig once, but this is probably due to High German influence.[350]
- ^ Cliticization of du is still observed in colloquial and dialectal German.[384]
- ^ Old High German, uniquely among attested Germanic languages, had a longer 1st person plural ending -mēs, e.g. (wir) nëmumēs "we take". Scholars are divided as to whether this ending goes back to similar-looking forms in Indo-European or represents the encliticization of the pronoun wir [nëmum wir > nëmumēs].This longer form had been replaced by -en by late Old High German.[399][400]
- ^ This narrow stretch with the unitary plural -en extends further to the northwest into the eastern part of the South Low Franconian Bergish dialect area.[407]
- ^ Foerste (1957) considers -ent to be a recent hybrid formation from the suffixes -en and -et, and not a continuation of -ent that appears in Middle Low German texts.[394]
- ^ In traditional German grammar, the present subjunctive is usually called "subjunctive I" (Konjunktiv I), whereas the past subjunctive is called "subjunctive II" (Konjunktiv II).[437]
- ^ Although Moselle Franconian had expanded the roots of gân and stân in the same manner as Standard German, Luxembourgish has recontracted them to gon, ston.[467]
- ^ The form of "to go" with e was originally an optative; in Old High German, beginning in Alemannic, the e form spread to the indicative. The very similar verb "to stand" was influenced by the the vocalism in "to go".[477]
- ^ In Middle Dutch, the form haven is attested in Eastern dialects; it derives from the same form as found in Old High German.[463]
- ^ Old High German shows a tendency to move -jan verbs to -ēn. This is reflected not only in the verb "to have" but also in some other verbs, such as sagēn vs. Old Saxon seggian and Old High German lebēn vs Old Saxon libbian, is an innovation.[355] Dutch also lacks libbian.[463]
- ^ Dutch shows a similar spread of umlaut/rounded front vowels in the former preterite present deugen from earlier dogen) and meugen (standard mogen) historically and in some dialects - other modals with the spelling ⟨u⟩ are already pronounced with /y/.[523][524]
Citations
[edit]- ^ Keel 2020.
- ^ Goossens 1970a, p. 113.
- ^ Seebold 2013, p. 73.
- ^ a b Goossens 1970a, p. 112.
- ^ a b Keel 2020, pp. 745–748.
- ^ a b Niebaum & Macha 2014, pp. 250–253.
- ^ Sanders 1974, p. 12.
- ^ a b Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 820–825.
- ^ a b c d Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 824. Cite error: The named reference "FOOTNOTEWiesinger1983a824" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ Niebaum & Macha 2014, pp. 250.
- ^ Marynissen & Janssens 2013, p. 85.
- ^ Kaiser 2021, p. 34.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 26.
- ^ Stiles 2013, pp. 19–20.
- ^ a b c Fulk 2018, p. 18.
- ^ a b c Van Bree 2013, p. 105.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 443.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 72.
- ^ a b Rübekeil 2017, p. 997.
- ^ Stiles 2013, p. 20.
- ^ Krogmann 1970, p. 236.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 443–444.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 44–446.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 146.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 145.
- ^ van Loon 2003, p. 141.
- ^ a b de Vaan 2017, p. 108.
- ^ a b c van Loon 2003, p. 145.
- ^ van Loon 2003, p. 166.
- ^ Versloot & Adamczyk 2017, p. 142.
- ^ Gallée 1993, pp. 153–154.
- ^ Versloot & Adamczyk 2017, p. 133.
- ^ van Loon 2003, pp. 147–148, 150–159.
- ^ Küpperbusch 1932.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 144.
- ^ van Loon 2003, pp. 159–166.
- ^ Stiles 2013, p. 18.
- ^ Fulk 2018, pp. 130–131.
- ^ Krogmann 1970, pp. 239–240.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 178.
- ^ Krogmann 1970, p. 240.
- ^ Lasch 1974, pp. 180–184.
- ^ van der Hoek 2010.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, p. 56, 114.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 141–142.
- ^ Krogmann 1970, p. 237.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, pp. 203–215.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1743.
- ^ Krogmann 1970, pp. 237–238.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 442–443.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, pp. 217–230.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 125.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 120.
- ^ a b c d Salmons 2018, p. 204.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, p. 81.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 136.
- ^ a b van der Hoek 2010, p. 3.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003, p. 120.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003, p. 119.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 131.
- ^ van Loey 1970, pp. 260–261.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 87–88.
- ^ Harbert 2007, p. 51.
- ^ Boersma 2017, p. 37.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, pp. 204–206.
- ^ Lasch 1974, pp. 131–132.
- ^ Boersma 2017, pp. 85–87.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 312–314.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003.
- ^ a b c Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, p. 104.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003, pp. 119–120.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003, pp. 122–123.
- ^ Goblirsch 2003, pp. 120–122.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 121.
- ^ Jones & Jones 2019, p. 65.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 162.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 162–167.
- ^ Laker 2014, pp. 264–266.
- ^ Laker 2014, pp. 275–283.
- ^ Laker 2014, pp. 283–291.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 132.
- ^ a b Simmler 1983, p. 1122.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 201–202.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2016, p. 240.
- ^ Goossens 1974, pp. 85–86.
- ^ Goossens 1974, pp. 97–98.
- ^ Frings & van Ginneken 1919, p. 168. Foerste 1957, p. 1805. Simmler 1983, pp. 1123–1124.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 202.
- ^ Hall 2021, p. 2.
- ^ Hall 2021, p. 6.
- ^ Taeldeman 2013b, p. 160.
- ^ Van Bree 2013, p. 115.
- ^ Hall 2021, p. 7.
- ^ Hall 2021, pp. 32–33.
- ^ Arens 1908, p. 10–11.
- ^ Polenz 2020, p. 93–94.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, p. 398–402.
- ^ Wagner 1926, p. 33.
- ^ a b van Loey 1970, p. 264.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, pp. 100–101.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 187.
- ^ a b Wagner 1926.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 156.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 466–469.
- ^ Frings 1961, p. 372.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 153.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, pp. 108–109.
- ^ Frings 1961, pp. 370–371.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 130.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 261–262.
- ^ Simmler 1983, p. 1222.
- ^ a b König 1994, p. 148.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 262.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 131.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 131–132.
- ^ a b c König 1994, p. 149.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 262–263.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 848.
- ^ a b Noelliste & Kniess 2024.
- ^ Kozianka & Sturm 2017, pp. 110, 115.
- ^ Taeldeman 2013b, pp. 159–160.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, map 33.
- ^ Hinskens 2009.
- ^ Werlen 1983, p. 1130.
- ^ Werlen 1983, pp. 1130–1131.
- ^ Noble 1983, p. 44, 53, 123.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1779–1780.
- ^ Werlen 1983, pp. 1131–1132.
- ^ Hermans 2013, pp. 344–345.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 857, 863.
- ^ Harbert 2007, p. 55.
- ^ Harbert 2007, pp. 55–57.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 301.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 301–302, 317–319.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 117–120.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 122–123.
- ^ Lahiri et al. 1999, p. 350.
- ^ Goossens 1974, pp. 73–74.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 66–67.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 145.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 145–146, 150, 168.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 522.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 454–455.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 431–432.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 146.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 455–456.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 435–436.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 437.
- ^ a b Foerste 1957, p. 1779.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 201.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 133.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 456.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 462.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 158–159.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 130.
- ^ Lüssy 1983, p. 1085.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 247–250.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 246–247, 292–296.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 132.
- ^ Howell & Salmons 1997, p. 92.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 62.
- ^ Harbert 2007, pp. 59–63.
- ^ a b c d Salmons 2017, p. 1008.
- ^ a b c Howell & Salmons 1997, p. 93.
- ^ Van Bree 2013, p. 106.
- ^ Howell & Salmons 1997, pp. 92–93.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, p. 131.
- ^ Goossens 1962, pp. 313–315.
- ^ Goossens 1970b, pp. 65–69.
- ^ Lüssy 1983, p. 1084.
- ^ Howell & Salmons 1997, p. 97.
- ^ Howell & Salmons 1997, p. 98.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 251.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 207.
- ^ Kroghe 2013, p. 158–159.
- ^ Versloot & Adamczyk 2017, p. 128–134.
- ^ a b c Taubken 1996, pp. 4–5.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, p. 505.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, p. 156.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1801–1802.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 47–49.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 288–289.
- ^ a b de Vaan 2017, p. 467.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 269–270.
- ^ a b Weijnen 1958, pp. 155–156.
- ^ a b Niebaum & Macha 2014, pp. 94–95.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 77.
- ^ Arens 1908, pp. 8–11.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 287-289.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 106–107.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 274.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970b, pp. 90–96.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, pp. 200.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 284–287.
- ^ a b c Fulk 2018, p. 76–77.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 110.
- ^ Keel 2020, p. 751.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970b, pp. 1–4, 58–59.
- ^ Noble 1983, p. 37.
- ^ Niebaum & Macha 2014, pp. 95.
- ^ Bloemhoff et al. 2013a, pp. 458–460.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, pp. 134–136.
- ^ a b Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 826–827.
- ^ Klein 2003b, p. 216.
- ^ Van Bree 2013, p. 111.
- ^ a b Boersma 2017, p. 49.
- ^ Polenz 2020, pp. 83–84.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 72.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2013, p. 114.
- ^ Swanenberg 2019, p. 3.
- ^ Hermans 2013, pp. 348–349.
- ^ Taubken 1996, p. 6. Niebaum 1980, p. 462. Foerste 1957, p. 1802
- ^ Fulk 2018, pp. 60–61, 76–78.
- ^ Goossens 1980, pp. 183–185. Wiesinger 1970a, p. 356. Niebaum & Macha 2014, p. 94
- ^ Goossens 1980, p. 196.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, p. 151.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 289–290. Wiesinger 1983a, p. 873, 879. Weijnen 1958, p. 151.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 356–359.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 874.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 290, 337.
- ^ Goossens 1962, p. 318.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 33–38.
- ^ Baechler & Pröll 2018, p. 3.
- ^ Fulk 2018, pp. 91–96.
- ^ van der Wal & Quak 1994, p. 74, 92.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 206.
- ^ König 1994, p. 73.
- ^ Klein 2003a, pp. 45–46.
- ^ Kienle 1969, p. 55.
- ^ Brogyani 1986, p. 86.
- ^ Baechler & Pröll 2018.
- ^ Brogyani 1986, pp. 84–86.
- ^ Goossens 1974, p. 55.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 80.
- ^ Marynissen 2009, p. 235-239.
- ^ König 1994, p. 159.
- ^ Keel 1981, p. 258.
- ^ Marynissen 2009, pp. 237–244.
- ^ Keel 2020, pp. 751–752.
- ^ a b Keel 1981, pp. 258–259.
- ^ Marynissen 2009, pp. 235–238.
- ^ Keel 1981, p. 259.
- ^ a b Ebert et al. 1993, pp. 79–80.
- ^ Franck 1910, pp. 21–25.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1809.
- ^ Keel 1981, pp. 259–261.
- ^ Harnisch 2019, p. 383.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 81–84.
- ^ a b Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 464.
- ^ Franck 1910, p. 54.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 218, 221.
- ^ Paul & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 71–73.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 36.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 78.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, pp. 37–38.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 258.
- ^ König 1994, pp. 148–149.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983b, p. 928.
- ^ De Schutter 2013, p. 287.
- ^ Hermans 2013, p. 348.
- ^ a b Goblirsch 2018, p. 37.
- ^ Goossens 2014, p. 144.
- ^ a b c Salmons 2018, p. 259.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1778.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 77.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 259–260.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983c, pp. 1103–1104.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, p. 102.
- ^ Van Bree 2013, p. 107.
- ^ Goossens 1970b, p. 71.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, pp. 154–155.
- ^ Goossens 1962, p. 323–324.
- ^ Taeldeman 2013a, pp. 212, 214.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983c, p. 1104.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1803.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 46.
- ^ Wiesinger 1970a, p. 49.
- ^ Examples taken from Henry (1900).
- ^ de Vaan 2017, p. 34.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, p. 78, 91.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 74.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, p. 91.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, p. 37, 252.
- ^ van Loey 1964, p. 30.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 92–93.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1770–1774.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 241–242, 256–258.
- ^ a b Goblirsch 2018, p. 75.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 254–256.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 97–98.
- ^ Seiler 2009.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 84–90.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 84–85, 94–95.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, pp. 84–89.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 256.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983d, p. 1092.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 233–235.
- ^ Goblirsch 2018, p. 97-98.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 235.
- ^ Lasch 1974, pp. 56–57.
- ^ a b c d Foerste 1957, p. 1775.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, p. 255.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 76–77.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, pp. 134–135.
- ^ Goossens 1974, p. 51.
- ^ van Loey 1964, p. 29.
- ^ a b c Salmons 2018, p. 257.
- ^ a b Wiesinger 1983e, pp. 1107–1109.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 248–252.
- ^ van Loey 1964, p. 93.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 298–304.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 250.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 298–305.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2016, pp. 350–351.
- ^ a b c d Goossens 1974, p. 50.
- ^ a b c van Loey 1964, p. 64-69.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 356–357.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 351.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 354.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2016, p. 355.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 350.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 351–352.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 353.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1774.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983d, pp. 1092–1093.
- ^ Haas 1983, pp. 1113–1114.
- ^ a b c Zhirmunski 2010, p. 431.
- ^ Goossens 1974, pp. 54–55.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 348–349.
- ^ a b Haas 1983, p. 1112.
- ^ Haas 1983, pp. 1111–1112.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 840.
- ^ a b Wiesinger 1983a, p. 891.
- ^ Boersma 2017, p. 27–29.
- ^ de Vaan 1999, p. 23–26.
- ^ Boersma 2017, p. 30.
- ^ de Vaan 1999, p. 26.
- ^ a b Boersma 2017, p. 31.
- ^ a b de Vaan 1999, p. 28.
- ^ Hermans 2013, p. 343.
- ^ Höder 2014, pp. 318–321.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 212–214.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 220–222.
- ^ a b Rabanus 2022.
- ^ Kienle 1969, p. 180.
- ^ Howe 1996, p. 105-107.
- ^ Fulk 2018, pp. 182–183.
- ^ Howe 1996, p. 255.
- ^ Lasch 1974, pp. 211–214.
- ^ a b Howe 1996, p. 207.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 267–268.
- ^ König 1994, p. 160.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 223–226.
- ^ Martin 1959, p. 46.
- ^ a b Foerste 1957, p. 1734.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 242.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 219.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 243–244.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 255–257, 268–269.
- ^ König 1994, p. 156, 160.
- ^ a b Wiesinger 1983a, p. 828.
- ^ Howe 1996, pp. 279–280.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 191.
- ^ Frings & Lerchner 1966, pp. 70–71, 77.
- ^ Fulk 2018, p. 188.
- ^ König 1994, p. 155.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2013, p. 116.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 148, 166–176.
- ^ Euler 2022, p. 171, 174.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 90.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1822.
- ^ Wiesinger 2017b, p. 206.
- ^ Kienle 1969, p. 289.
- ^ Paul 1968, p. 193.
- ^ a b Newton 1990, p. 176.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 850, 857.
- ^ Nübling 2001, pp. 443–444.
- ^ Franck 1910, pp. 115–116.
- ^ van Loey 1970, p. 273.
- ^ a b Weijnen 1958, p. 208–209.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 80, 90.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, p. 208.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 91.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 150–151.
- ^ a b Fulk 2018, p. 274.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, p. 747-748.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 587–588.
- ^ a b Donaldson 1983, pp. 174–175.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 92–93.
- ^ Durrell 1990, p. 82.
- ^ Panzer 1972, pp. 148–149.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 225.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 612.
- ^ a b c Foerste 1957, p. 1823. Cite error: The named reference "FOOTNOTEFoerste19571823" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ Kienle 1969, p. 290.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 81-83.
- ^ Rein 1983, p. 1150.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 103–104.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 260–262.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 82, 151.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 824, 835. Koch 2019, p. 293. Lenz 2019, p. 331.
- ^ Christen 2019, p. 259.
- ^ a b Münch 1904, pp. 168, 184.
- ^ Stiles 2013, pp. 17–18.
- ^ Lasch 1974, pp. 226–227.
- ^ a b Härd 1980, p. 588.
- ^ Wiesinger 2017a, p. 345.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 824. Bloemhoff et al. 2013b, pp. 486–487. Rabanus 2005, p. 270.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 824. Niebaum & Macha 2014, pp. 250. Marynissen & Janssens 2013, p. 85.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 240.
- ^ Franck 1910, pp. 113–114.
- ^ Van Bree 2013, p. 118.
- ^ a b c d Weijnen 1958, p. 209.
- ^ a b Rabanus 2005, p. 270.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, pp. 834–835, 848.
- ^ a b c Van Bree 2020, pp. 104–105.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 587.
- ^ Weijnen 1958, p. 207.
- ^ Durrell 1990, pp. 81–82.
- ^ a b c Weijnen 1958, p. 206.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2020, p. 103.
- ^ a b c d Foerste 1957, p. 1828.
- ^ a b Niebaum 1983, p. 173.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, pp. 214–215.
- ^ Gaeta 2010, pp. 92–96.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, pp. 176–177.
- ^ Fertig 1998, pp. 261–262; Schockaert & Van de Velde 2024; Hol 1941, p. 251
- ^ Schockaert & Van de Velde 2024, p. 225-227.
- ^ Fertig 1998, pp. 262–265.
- ^ Keel 1981, p. 260.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 875.
- ^ Wiesinger 1983a, p. 884.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, pp. 284.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 562–563.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 604.
- ^ Van Bree 2016, p. 239.
- ^ a b Dammel 2011, p. 168.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 148–149, 166–174.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 260–261.
- ^ Saltveit 1983, pp. 1222–1223.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 150–152, 176–178.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, pp. 252–253.
- ^ van Loey 1964, p. 174.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 241.
- ^ Lasch 1974, p. 228.
- ^ Lüssy 1983, p. 1087.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 270.
- ^ Dammel 2011, pp. 168–170.
- ^ Dammel 2011, pp. 170–172.
- ^ Dammel 2011, p. 172.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 112–113.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1787–1788, 1824, 1827.
- ^ Durrell 1990, p. 83.
- ^ Panzer 1972, pp. 151–152.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, p. 916.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 307–312.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 184–192.
- ^ Dammel 2011, pp. 174–175.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, pp. 915–916.
- ^ Nübling 1995a, p. 169-170.
- ^ Dammel 2011, pp. 174–176.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, pp. 946–971.
- ^ a b c d Van Bree 2020, p. 117. Cite error: The named reference "FOOTNOTEVan Bree2020117" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ Nübling 2001, p. 456.
- ^ Nübling 1995b, pp. 132–133.
- ^ Nübling 1995b, p. 138.
- ^ Nübling 2001, pp. 456–457.
- ^ Newton 1990, p. 176-179.
- ^ De Schutter & Hermans 2013, p. 366.
- ^ Nübling 1995a, pp. 170–171.
- ^ Nübling 1995a, p. 172.
- ^ a b Nübling 1995a, p. 166. Cite error: The named reference "FOOTNOTENübling1995a166" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 628–630.
- ^ Nübling 1995b.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 79–80.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2020, pp. 84–85.
- ^ a b Euler 2022, pp. 191–192.
- ^ Noble 1983, pp. 37–38.
- ^ a b Martin 1959, pp. 70–71.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, p. 472.
- ^ a b c Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 626–627.
- ^ Klein & Büthe 2011, p. 305–308.
- ^ a b Foerste 1957, p. ?.
- ^ Müller 1931, p. 1100, entry gehen
- ^ Behaghel 1928, p. 473.
- ^ Nübling 1995a, pp. 166–168.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 84.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 627–628.
- ^ Meisen 1964, p. 1444, entry tun
- ^ a b Zhirmunski 2010, p. 629.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 114–115.
- ^ Martin 1959, p. 71.
- ^ a b Donaldson 1983, pp. 182–183.
- ^ a b Rein 1983, p. 1151.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, pp. 481–482.
- ^ Grimm 2023, pp. 10:45ff..
- ^ Grimm 2023, p. 10:45ff.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1828–1829.
- ^ a b De Schutter 2008, pp. 141–142.
- ^ Frings 1916, pp. 212–213, 239.
- ^ Goossens 1965, pp. 82–84, 91.
- ^ Hermans 2013, pp. 337–338.
- ^ Frings & van Ginneken 1919, pp. 149–151; Map 2.
- ^ Grimm 2023, pp. 12:213ff..
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 66.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 178–184.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 303–304.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 153–155.
- ^ a b Dammel 2011, pp. 141–142.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 193–194.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, p. 306.
- ^ Brinkmann 1987, p. 161.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, p. 891.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2020, p. 73.
- ^ Braune & Reiffenstein 2004, pp. 304–305.
- ^ Klein, Solms & Wegera 2018, p. 900.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, p. 484.
- ^ a b Niebaum 1983, p. 172.
- ^ a b Van Bree 2020, p. 77.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 622.
- ^ a b Zhirmunski 2010, p. 623.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 72.
- ^ a b c d e van Loey 1964, p. 146.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 259–261.
- ^ Nübling 2008, pp. 96–103.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 74.
- ^ Dammel & Schallert 2020, p. 206.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, pp. 483–484.
- ^ Dammel & Schallert 2020, pp. 207–208.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, pp. 616–619.
- ^ Behaghel 1928, pp. 482–483.
- ^ a b Grimm 2023, pp. 16:1452ff..
- ^ Nübling 2008, pp. 103–104.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 616.
- ^ a b Foerste 1957, p. 1830.
- ^ Zhirmunski 2010, p. 620.
- ^ Fritz 1997, p. 10-11.
- ^ Werlen 1985, p. 7.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 75.
- ^ Fritz 1997, pp. 10–11.
- ^ Fritz 1997, pp. 9–10.
- ^ Fritz 1997, p. 9.
- ^ Nübling 2008, p. 94.
- ^ Grimm 2023, pp. 12:2748ff..
- ^ Fritz 1997, p. 10.
- ^ Van Bree 2007, p. 231.
- ^ van Loey 1964, p. 147.
- ^ a b Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 263.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1829.
- ^ a b Fritz 1997, p. 11.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, pp. 180–181.
- ^ Grimm 2023, pp. 30:1326ff..
- ^ Weiß 2005, p. 149, 152-153.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, pp. 336–337.
- ^ van Koppen 2017, p. 5.
- ^ Weiß 2005, pp. 149–150.
- ^ a b c van Koppen 2017, pp. 7–8.
- ^ Weiß 2005, pp. 151–152.
- ^ Weiß 2005, p. 152.
- ^ Kürschner & Nübling 2011, p. 369, 370.
- ^ a b Havranová 2014, p. 165.
- ^ Harbert 2007, p. 103-104.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 322.
- ^ Shrier 1965, p. 423.
- ^ Van de Velde 2024.
- ^ Harbert 2007, pp. 104–105.
- ^ Panzer 1972, pp. 152–153.
- ^ Keel 2020, p. 753.
- ^ Shrier 1965, pp. 431–435.
- ^ Panzer 1972, p. 152-153, 155, 161.
- ^ Scott 2014, p. 118.
- ^ Panzer 1972, pp. 153–154.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 324–325.
- ^ Kürschner & Nübling 2011, pp. 375–376.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, p. 398.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 188.
- ^ Ebert et al. 1993, pp. 172–173.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, pp. 411–412.
- ^ a b c Dingeldein 1983, p. 1199.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 162–163.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, pp. 401–402.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, p. 217, 219, 264-265.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 265.
- ^ Kürschner & Nübling 2011, pp. 367–368.
- ^ a b c Dingeldein 1983, p. 1198.
- ^ a b Versloot 2016.
- ^ Foerste 1957, pp. 1781–1782.
- ^ a b Kürschner & Nübling 2011, p. 376.
- ^ Dingeldein 1983, pp. 1197–1199.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 106–107.
- ^ Klein 2013.
- ^ a b Salmons 2018, pp. 265–266.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 325.
- ^ Klein 2013, pp. 175.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 165.
- ^ a b c d Dingeldein 1983, p. 1200.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1811.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, pp. 415–416.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, pp. 164–165.
- ^ a b Adamczyk 2022, p. 416. Cite error: The named reference "FOOTNOTEAdamczyk2022416" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ a b c d Dingeldein 1983, p. 1999.
- ^ Dingeldein 1983, pp. 1999–1200.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, p. 164.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, p. 166.
- ^ Euler 2022, p. 79.
- ^ Versloot 2016, pp. 466.
- ^ Voyles 1971, p. 143.
- ^ Foerste 1957, p. 1782.
- ^ a b Fischer 2024, p. 261.
- ^ a b van der Wal & Quak 1994, p. 75.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, p. 409.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 155.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, pp. 409–410.
- ^ de Vaan 2017, pp. 30, 83–84.
- ^ Adamczyk 2022, p. 414.
- ^ Salmons 2018, pp. 300–301.
- ^ Salmons 2018, p. 323.
- ^ Euler 2022, pp. 77–78.
- ^ Van Bree 2020, p. 160.
- ^ Paul, Wiehl & Grosse 1998, p. 190.
- ^ Kürschner 2008, p. 106.
- ^ Donaldson 1983, p. 165.
- ^ Lüssy 1983, p. 1086.
- ^ Dingeldein 1983, pp. 1197–1198.
- ^ Niebaum 1983, p. 160.
- ^ a b König 1994, p. 157.
- ^ Tiefenbach 1987, p. 8.
- ^ Tiefenbach 1987, p. 9.
- ^ Tiefenbach 1987, pp. 19–25.
- ^ Tiefenbach 1987, pp. 13–15.
- ^ Lameli 2018.
- ^ a b Sanders 1974, p. 11.
- ^ Sanders 1974, p. 17.
- ^ Sanders 1974, p. 18.
- ^ Harbert 2007, pp. 16–17.
Bibliography
[edit]- Adamczyk, Elżbieta (2022). "Plural inflection in varieties of Dutch: Patterns of restructuring and geographical redistribution". Nederlandse Taalkunde. 27 (3): 394–427. doi:10.5117/NEDtAA2022.3.005.ADAM.
- Arens, Josef (1908). Der Vokalismus der Mundarten im Kreise Olpe unter Zugrundelegung der Mundart von Elspe. Borna: Robert Noske.
- Baechler, Raffaela; Pröll, Simon (2018). "Loss and preservation of case in Germanic non-standard varieties". Glossa: a journal of general linguistics. 3 (1:): 1–35. doi:10.5334/gjgl.680.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - Beckmann, Werner (1990). "Zur Geschichte der deutschen Modalverben. Das Problem des Umlaüs bei den Modalverben in der deutschen Schiftsprache und den Dialekten" (PDF). Niederdeutsches Wort. 30: 55–81.
- Behaghel, Otto (1928). Geschichte der deutschen Sprache. Grundriß der germanischen Philologie, Band 3 (5th ed.). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
- Bloemhoff, Henk; Niebaum, Hermann; Twilhaar, Jan Nijen; Scholtmeijer, Harrie (2013a). "Low Saxon phonology". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 454–475. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.454.
- Bloemhoff, Henk; Niebaum, Hermann; Twilhaar, Jan Nijen; Scholtmeijer, Harrie (2013b). "The Low Saxon dialects: Morphology and syntax". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 476–495. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.476.
- Boersma, Paul (2017). "The history of the Franconian tone contrast". In Wolfgang Kehrein; Björn Köhnlein; Paul Boersma; Marc van Oostendorp (eds.). Segmental Structure and Tone (PDF). Berlin: De Gruyter. pp. 27–97. doi:10.1515/9783110341263-003. ISBN 978-3-11-034126-3.
- Braune, Wilhelm; Reiffenstein, Ingo (2004). Althochdeutsche Grammatik (15 ed.). Tübingen: Niemeyer.
- Brinkmann, Thomas (1987). Präteritopräsentia. Morphologische Entwicklungen einer Sonderklasse in den altgermanischen Sprachen. Niemeyer. doi:10.1515/9783111357706.
- Brogyani, Bela (1986). "Nicht abgeschwächte Endsilbenvokale in spätmittelhochdeutscher Zeit: Untersuchungen anhand des großen Urbars von Einsiedeln aus dem Jahre 1331". In Klingenberg, Heinz; Brogyanyi, Bela; Krömmelbein, Thomas (eds.). Germanic dialects : linguistic and philological investigations. J. Benjamins. pp. 81–107. ISBN 9027235260.
- Christen, Helen (2019). "Alemannisch In Der Schweiz". In Joachim Herrgen; Jürgen Erich Schmidt (eds.). Sprache und Raum - Ein internationales Handbuch der Sprachvariation. Band 4: Deutsch. Handbücher Zur Sprach- Und Kommunikationswissenschaft. 30.4. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 246–279. doi:10.1515/9783110261295-09.
- Dammel, Antje (2011). Konjugationsklassenwandel. Prinzipien des Ab-, Um- und Ausbaus verbalflexivischer Allomorphie in germanischen Sprachen. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110240351.
- Dammel, Antje; Schallert, Oliver (2020). "Modalverben in deutschen Dialekten: Ein Testfall für die Analyse morphologischer Variation". In Christen, Helen; et al. (eds.). Regiolekt - der neue Dialekt? Akten des 6. Kongresses der Internationalen Gesellschaft für Dialektologie des Deutschen (IGDD). Franz Steiner Verlag. pp. 193–226.
- De Schutter, Georges (2008). "Werkwoordvormen in de zuidelijke Nederlandse dialecten: stamkeuze en werkwoorduitgangen. Analyse en beschrijving op basis van het RND-materiaal, deel I" (PDF). Taal en Tongval (60): 121–171.
- De Schutter, Georges (2013). "The dialects of the Brabant region: Phonological properties". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 277–297. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.277.
- De Schutter, Georges; Hermans, Ben (2013). "The Limburg dialects: Grammatical properties". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 356–377. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.356.
- de Vaan, Michiel (1999). "Towards an explanation of the Franconian tone accents". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 51: 23–44. doi:10.1163/18756719-051-01-90000004. hdl:1887/14124.
- de Vaan, Michiel (2017). The Dawn of Dutch: Language contact in the Western Low Countries before 1200. John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/nss.30.
- Dingeldein, Heinrich J. (1983). "Spezielle Pluralbildungen in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1196–1202. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-006.
- Donaldson, B.C. (1983). Dutch: A Linguistic History of Holland and Belgium. Martinus Nijhoff.
- Durrell, Martin (1990). "Westphalian and Eastphalian". In Russ, Charles V. J. (ed.). The Dialects of Modern German: A Linguistic Survey. Routledge. pp. 59–89.
- Ebert, Robert P.; Reichmann, Oskar; Solms, Hans-Joachim; Wegera, Klaus-Peter (1993). Ebert, Robert P.; Reichmann, Oskar; Solms, Hans-Joachim; Wegera, Klaus-Peter (eds.). Frühneuhochdeutsche Grammatik. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110920130. ISBN 978-3-484-10672-7.
- Euler, Wolfram (2022). Das Westgermanische. Seine Rekonstruktion von der Herausbildung im 3. Jahrhundert bis zur Aufgliederung im 7. Jahrhundert (2 ed.). Verlag Inspiration Unlimited. ISBN 978-945127-414.
{{cite book}}
: Check|isbn=
value: length (help) - Fertig, David (1998). "The ge- Participle Prefix in Early New High German and the Modern Dialects". American Journal of Germanic Linguistics & Literatures. 10 (2): 237–78. doi:10.1017/S1040820700002353.
- Fischer, Hanna (2021). "Präteritumschwund im Deutschen. Neue Erkenntnisse zu einem alten Rätsel". Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur. 143 (3): 331–363. doi:10.1515/bgsl-2021-0027.
- Fischer, Hanna (2024). "Von der Flexion in die Wortbildung und darüber hinaus. Zur diachronen Entwicklung des s-Suffixes im Deutschen". Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik. 52 (2): 257–286. doi:10.1515/zgl-2024-2013.
- Foerste, William (1957). "Geschichte der niederdeutschen Mundarten". In Wolfgang Stammler (ed.). Deutsche Philologie im Aufriß. Vol. 1 (2nd ed.). Berlin: Erich Schmidt. pp. 1729–1898.
- Franck, Johannes (1910). Mittelniederländische Grammatik (2 ed.). Tauchnitz.
- Frings, Theodor (1916). "Mittelfränkisch-niedererfränkische Studien". Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur. 41: 193–271. doi:10.1515/bgsl.1916.1916.41.193.
- Frings, Theodor (1961). "FLÄMISCH KACHTEL 'FÜLLEN', LATEINISCH CAPITALE, UND DER ÜBERGANG VON FT ZU CHT, DEUTSCH KRAFT, NIEDERLÄNDISCH CRACHT". Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur. 81 (Sonderband): 363–393. doi:10.1515/bgsl.1961.82.s1.363.
- Frings, Theodor; Lerchner, Gotthard (1966). Niederländisch und Niederdeutsch: Aufbau und Gliederung des Niederdeutschen. Akademie Verlag. doi:10.1515/9783112701034.
- Frings, Theodor; van Ginneken, Jacob (1919). "Zur Geschichte des Niederfränkischen in Limburg". Zeitschrift für Deutsche Mundarten. 14: 97–209. JSTOR 40497953.
- Fritz, Gerd (1997). "Historische Semantik der Modalverben. Problemskizze – Exemplarische Analysen – Forschungsüberblick. 1. Einleitung und erste Orientierung". In Fritz, Gerd; Gloning, Thomas (eds.). Untersuchungen zur semantischen Entwicklungsgeschichte der Modalverben im Deutschen. Niemyer. pp. 1–13. doi:10.1515/9783110940848.1.
- Fulk, R.D. (2018). A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages. Studies in Germanic Linguistics. Vol. 3. John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/sigl.3. ISBN 978-90-272-6312-4.
- Gaeta, Livio (2010). "The Invisible Hand of Grammaticalization: West Germanic substitutive infinitive and the prefix ge-". In Rainer, Franz (ed.). Variation and Change in Morphology. Selected Papers from the 13 International Morphology Meeting, Vienne, February 2008. John Benjamins. pp. 89–105.
- Gallée, Johan Hendrik (1993). Altsächsische Grammatik. Mit Berichtigungen und Literaturnachträgen. Nach Wendelin Försters letzter Ausgabe in Auswahl bearbeitet und mit Einleitung und Glossar versehen (3 ed.). Niemeyer. doi:10.1515/9783110920147.
- Goblirsch, Kurt Gustav (2003). "THE VOICING OF FRICATIVES IN WEST GERMANIC AND THE PARTIAL CONSONANT SHIFT". Folia Linguistica Historica. 37 (1–2): 111–152. doi:10.1515/flih.2003.24.1-2.111.
- Goblirsch, Kurt Gustav (2018). Gemination, Lenition, and Vowel Lengthening: On the History of Quantity in Germanic. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781139540780.
- Goossens, Jan (1962). "Die Gerundeten Palatalvokale Im Niederländischen Sprachraum". Zeitschrift Für Mundartforschung. 29 (4): 312–328. JSTOR 40500045.
- Goossens, Jan (1965). "Die Gliederung des Südniederfränkischen" (PDF). Rheinische Vierteljahrsblätter. 30: 79–94.
- Goossens, Jan (1970a). "Inleiding tot de Nederlandse dialectologie". Handelingen van de Koninklijke Commissie voor Toponymie en Dialectologie. 44 (1): 105–273. doi:10.21825/hctd.88272.
- Goossens, Jan (1970b). "Niederländische Mundarten – vom Deutschen aus gesehen" (PDF). Niederdeutsches Wort. 10: 61–80.
- Goossens, Jan (1974). Historische Phonologie des Niederländischen. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783111411576.
- Goossens, Jan (1980). "Middelnederlandse vocaalsystemen" (PDF). Verslagen en Mededelingen van de Koninklijke Academie voor Nederlandse Taal- en Letterkunde: 161–251.
- Goossens, Jan (1987). "Schets van de meervoudsvorming der substantieven in de Nederlandse dialecten". Taal & Tongval. 39: 141–174.
- Goossens, Jan (2008). "Dialectgeografische grondslagen van een Nederlandse taalgeschiedenis". Handelingen van de Koninklijke Commissie voor Toponymie en Dialectologie. 80 (1): 33–258. doi:10.21825/hctd.88678.
- Goossens, Jan (1994). "Ze den Personalpronomina im Rhein- und Maasland". In Mattheier, Klaus J.; Wiesinger, Peter (eds.). Dialektologie des Deutschen. Forschungsstand und Entwicklungstendenzen. de Gruyter. pp. 45–48. doi:10.1515/9783110958485.45.
- Goossens, Jan (2014). "Het vroegere Zuid-Brabantse ontrondingsgebied". Handelingen van de Koninklijke Commissie voor Toponymie en Dialectologie. 86: 139–152. doi:10.2143/TD.86.0.1000009.
- Grimm, Jacob (2023). Deutsches Wörterbuch von Jacob Grimm und Wilhelm Grimm, digitalisierte Fassung im Wörterbuchnetz des Trier Center for Digital Humanities.
- Grimme, Hubert (1910). Plattdeutsche Mundarten. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783111367583.
- Haas, Walter (1983). "Vokalisierung in den deutschen Dialekten". Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1111–1116. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-005.
- Harbert, Wayne (2007). The Germanic Languages. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511755071.
- Härd, John Evert (1980). "Mittelniederdeutsch". In Hans Peter Althaus; Helmut Henne; Herbert Ernst Wiegand (eds.). Lexikon der Germanistischen Linguistik. Berlin, New York: Max Niemeyer. pp. 584–588. doi:10.1515/9783110960846.584.
- Hall, Tracy Alan (2021). "The Realization of West Germanic +[sk] in Low German". Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur. 143 (1): 1–50. doi:10.1515/bgsl-2021-0001.
- Harnisch, Rüdiger (2019). "Ostfränkisch". Sprache und Raum - Ein internationales Handbuch der Sprachvariation. Band 4: Deutsch. de Gruyter Mouton. pp. 363–406. doi:10.1515/9783110261295-012.
- Havranová, Kateřina (2014). "Default Case in Dutch: A Comparative Study of Dutch and English Case Systems". In Ludmila Veselovská; Markéta Janebová (eds.). Complex Visibles Out There. Proceedings of the Olomouc Linguistics Colloquium 2014: Language Use and Linguistic Structure. Palacký University. pp. 163–174.
- Henry, Victor (1900). Le dialecte alaman de Colmar (Haute-Alsace) en 1870: grammaire et lexique. Paris: Félix Alcan.
- Hermans, Ben (2013). "Phonological features of Limburgian dialects". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 336–355. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.336.
- Hinskens, Frans (2009). "The erosion of a variable process. The case of n-deletion in Ripuarian and Limburg dialects of Dutch". In Kügler, Frank; Féry, Caroline; van de Vijver, Ruben (eds.). Variation and Gradience in Phonetics and Phonology. de Gruyter Mouton. pp. 311–350. doi:10.1515/9783110219326.
- Höder, Steffen (2014). "Low German: A profile of a word language". In Reina, Javier Caro; Szczepaniak, Renata (eds.). Syllable and Word Languages. de Gruyter. pp. 305–326. doi:10.1515/9783110346992.305.
- Hol, A. R. (1941). "Het prefix in het verleden deelwoord". Tijdschrift voor Nederlandse taal en letterkunde. 60: 249–293.
- Howe, Stephen (1996). The Personal Pronouns in the Germanic Languages: A Study of Personal Pronoun Morphology and Change in the Germanic Languages from the First Records to the Present Day. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110819205.
- Hooge, David (1983). "Verwendungstypen der Tempusformen in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1209–1220. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-006.
- Howell, Robert B.; Salmons, Joseph (1997). "Umlautless residues in Germanic". American Journal of Germanic Linguistics & Literatures. 9 (1): 83–111. doi:10.1017/S1040820700002006.
- Jones, Howard; Jones, Martin H. (2019). The Oxford Guide to Middle High German. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oso/9780199654611.001.0001.
- Kaiser, Livia (2021). Runes Across the North Sea from the Migration Period and Beyond: An Annotated Edition of the Old Frisian Runic Corpus. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110728224.
- Keel, William D. (1981). "APOCOPE AND SYNCOPE IN MODERN GERMAN DIALECTS". In Henderson, Michael M. T. (ed.). 1980 Mid-America Linguistics Conference papers. University of Kansas. pp. 257–265.
- Keel, William D. (2020). "The West Germanic Dialect Continuum". In Michael T. Putnam; B. Richard Page (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Germanic Linguistics. Cambridge Handbooks in Language and Linguistics. Cambridge University Press. pp. 736–760. doi:10.1017/9781108378291.032.
- Kienle, Richard (1969). Historische Laut- und Formenlehre des Deutschen. de Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9783111392479.
- Klein, Thomas (2003a). "ALTHOCHDEUTSCH UND ALTNIEDERLÄNDISCH". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 57 (1): 19–60. doi:10.1163/18756719-90000131.
- Klein, Thomas (2003b). "Niederdeutsch und Hochdeutsch in mittelhochdeutscher Zeit". In Berthele, Raphael; Christen, Helen; Germann, Sibylle; Hove, Ingrid (eds.). Die deutsche Schriftsprache und die Regionen: Entstehungsgeschichtliche Fragen in neuer Sicht. de Gruyter. pp. 203–229. doi:10.1515/9783110201574.203.
- Klein, Thomas; Büthe, Eva (2011). "Regularisierung des Irregulären Zur Geschichte der Verbgruppe um gehen und stehen im Mittelfränkischen". In Plate, Ralf; Schubert, Martin (eds.). Mittelhochdeutsch. Beiträge zur Überlieferung, Sprache und Literatur. de Gruyter. pp. 305–330. doi:10.1515/9783110262353.305.
- Klein, Thomas (2013). "Zum r-Plural im Westgermanischen". NOWELE. North-Western European Language Evolution. 66 (2): 169–196. doi:10.1075/nowele.66.2.03kle.
- Klein, Thomas; Solms, Hans-Joachim; Wegera, Klaus-Peter (2018). Mittelhochdeutsche Grammatik. Teil II: Flexionsmorphologie. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110523522.
- Koch, Günter (2019). "Bairisch In Deutschland". In Joachim Herrgen; Jürgen Erich Schmidt (eds.). Sprache und Raum - Ein internationales Handbuch der Sprachvariation. Band 4: Deutsch. Handbücher Zur Sprach- Und Kommunikationswissenschaft. 30.4. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 279–318. doi:10.1515/9783110261295-010.
- König, Werner (1994). dtv-Atlas zur deutschen Sprache (10 ed.). Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag.
- Kozianka, Maria; Sturm, Laura (2017). "Prothese und Aphärese im Westgermanischen". Jahrbuch für Germanistische Sprachgeschichte. 8 (1): 108–120. doi:10.1515/jbgsg-2017-0008.
- Krogmann, Willy (1970). "ALTSÄCHSISCH UND MITTELNIEDERDEUTSCH". In Schmitt, Ludwig E. (ed.). Kurzer Grundriß der germanischen Philologie bis 1500. Band 1: Sprachgeschichte. de Gruyter. pp. 211–252. doi:10.1515/9783110822717.211.
- Kroghe, Steffen (2013). "Die Anfänge des Altsächsischen". NOWELE. 66 (2): 141–168. doi:10.1075/nowele.66.2.02kro.
- Küpperbusch, Emil (1932). "Born und Brunnen". Teuthonista. 8 (1/2): 55–94. JSTOR 40498822.
- Kürschner, Sebastian (2008). Deklinationsklassen-Wandel. Eine diachron-kontrastive Studie zur Entwicklung der Pluralallomorphie im Deutschen, Niederländischen, Schwedischen und Dänischen. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110210842.
- Kürschner, Sebastian (2018). "Dialects of German, Dutch, and the Scandinavian Languages". In Charles Boberg; John Nerbonne; Dominic Watt (eds.). The Handbook of Dialectology. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell. pp. 462–473. doi:10.1002/9781118827628.ch27.
- Kürschner, Sebastian; Nübling, Damaris (2011). "The interaction of gender and declension in Germanic languages". Folia Linguistica. 45 (2): 355–388. doi:10.1515/flin.2011.014.
- Laker, Stephen (2014). "The Downfall of Dental Fricatives: Frisian Perspectives on a Wider Germanic Trend". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 73: 261–300. doi:10.1163/9789401211918_011.
- Lahiri, Aditi; Riad, Tomas; Jacobs, Haike (1999). "Diachronic prosody". In van der Hulst, Harry (ed.). Word Prosodic Systems in the Languages of Europe. de Gruyter Mouton. pp. 335–422. doi:10.1515/9783110197082.1.335.
- Lahiri, Aditi; Dresher, B. Elan (1999). "Open Syllable Lengthening in West Germanic". Language. 75 (4): 678–719. JSTOR 417730.
- Lameli, Alfred (2018). "The replacement of diminutive suffixes in the New High German period: A time series analysis in word formation". Journal of Historical Linguistics. 8 (2): 273–313. doi:10.1075/jhl.17014.lam.
- Lasch, Agathe (1974). Mittelniederdeutsche Grammatik (2 ed.). Niemeyer. doi:10.1515/9783111393124.
- Lenz, Alexandra N. (2019). "Bairisch Und Alemannisch In Österreich". In Joachim Herrgen; Jürgen Erich Schmidt (eds.). Sprache und Raum - Ein internationales Handbuch der Sprachvariation. Band 4: Deutsch. Handbücher Zur Sprach- Und Kommunikationswissenschaft. 30.4. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 318–363. doi:10.1515/9783110261295-011.
- Lüssy, Heinrich (1983). "Umlautung in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1083–1088. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-005.
- Marynissen, Ann (2009). "Taalverandering tussen evolutie en normering: De e-apocope als breuklijn tussen het Nederlands en het Duits". Nederlandse Taalkunde. 14 (3): 233–254.
- Marynissen, Ann; Janssens, Guy (2013). "A regional history of Dutch". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 81–100. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.81.
- Martin, Bernhard (1959). Die deutschen Mundarten (2 ed.). N. G. Elwert Verlag.
- Meisen, Karl, ed. (1964). Rheinisches Wörterbuch, Band 8. Berlin: Erika Klopp.
- Mitzka, Walther (1957). "Hochdeutsche Mundarten". In Wolfgang Stammler (ed.). Deutsche Philologie im Aufriß. Vol. 1 (2nd ed.). Berlin: Erich Schmidt. pp. 1599–1728.
- Müller, Josef, ed. (1931). Rheinisches Wörterbuch, Band 2. Berlin: Fritz Klopp.
- Münch, Ferdinand (1904). Grammatik der ripuarisch-fränkischen Mundart. Bonn: Bouvier.
- Newton, G. (1990). "Central Franconian". In Russ, Charles V. J. (ed.). The Dialects of Modern German: A Linguistic Survey. Routledge. pp. 136–209.
- Niebaum, Hermann (1980). "Westniederdeutsch". In Hans Peter Althaus; Helmut Henne; Herbert Ernst Wiegand (eds.). Lexikon der Germanistischen Linguistik. Berlin, New York: Max Niemeyer. pp. 458–464. doi:10.1515/9783110960846.458.
- Niebaum, Hermann (1983). "Zur Formengeographie". In Goossens, Jan (ed.). Niederdeutsch. Sprache und Literatur (2 ed.). pp. 158–174.
- Niebaum, Hermann; Macha, Jürgen (2014). Enführung in die Dialektologie des Deutschen. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110338713.
- Nielsen, Hans Frede (2001). "Frisian and the Grouping of the Older Germanic Languages". In Munske, Horst Haider; Århammar, Nils; Faltings, Volker F.; Hoekstra, Jarich F.; Vries, Oebele; Walker, Alastair G.H.; Wilts, Ommo (eds.). Handbuch des Friesischen. Niemeyer. pp. 512–523. doi:10.1515/9783110946925.512.
- Noelliste, Erin; Kniess, Tyler (2024). "Unmarkedness of the coronal nasal in Alemannic". The Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics. 27 (7). doi:10.1007/s10828-024-09153-x.
- Noble, C.A.M. (1983). Modern German dialects. Peter Long.
- Nübling, Damaris (1995a). "Die Kurzverben im Schweizerdeutschen : in der Kürze liegt die Würze oder: Im Spannungsfeld zwischen Reduktion und Differenzierung". In Löffler, Heinrich (ed.). Alemannische Dialektforschung : Bilanz und Perspektiven ; Beiträge zur 11. Arbeitstagung Alemannischer Dialektologen (PDF). Francke. pp. 165–179.
- Nübling, Damaris (1995b). "Kurzverben in germanischen Sprachen. Unterschiedliche Wege - gleiche Ziele". Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik. 62 (2): 127–154. JSTOR 40504008.
- Nübling, Damaris (2000). Prinzipien der Irregularisierung. Eine kontrastive Analyse von zehn Verben in zehn germanischen Sprachen. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110915082.
- Nübling, Damaris (2001). "Wechselflexion Luxemburgisch - Deutsch kontrastiv: "ech soen - du sees/si seet" vs. "ich sage, du sagst, sie sagt" : zum sekundären Ausbau eines präsentischen Wurzelvokalwechsels im Luxemburgischen" (PDF). Sprachwissenschaft. 26 (4): 433–472.
- Nübling, Damaris (2008). "Müssen, dürfen, können, mögen: Wie kam der Umlaut in die Prateritoprasentia? - Neues zu einem alten Problem der Irregularitat". In Stroh, Cornelia; Urdze, Aina (eds.). Morphologische Irregularität. Neue Ansätze, Sichtweisen und Daten. Universitätsverlag Dr. N. Brockmeyer. pp. 91–109.
- Panzer, Baldur (1972). "Morphologische Systeme niederdeutscher und niederländischer Dialekte" (PDF). Niederdeutsches Wort. 12: 144–169.
- Paul, Hermann (1968). Deutsche Grammatik. de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110929805.
- Paul, Hermann; Wiehl, Peter; Grosse, Siegfried (1998). Mittelhochdeutsche Grammatik (24 ed.). Niemeyer.
- Polenz, Peter von (2020). Geschichte der deutschen Sprache (11 ed.). de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110485660.
- Rabanus, Stefan (2005). "Dialektwandel im 20. Jahrhundert. Verbalplural in Südwestdeutschland". In Eggers, Eckhard; Schmidt, Jürgen Erich; Stellmacher, Dieter (eds.). Moderne Dialekte – Neue Dialektologie. Akten des 1. Kongresses der Internationalen Gesellschaft für Dialektologie des Deutschen (IGDD) am Forschungsinstitut für deutsche Sprache „Deutscher Sprachatlas“ der Philipps-Universität Marburg vom 5.–8. März 2003. Stuttgart: Steiner. pp. 267–290. hdl:11562/23971.
- Rabanus, Stefan (2022). "Nominal Inflectional Morphology in Germanic: Pronouns". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.952.
- Rein, Kurt (1983). "Kontraktion in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1147–1154. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-006.
- Roos, N.G.J. (2009). The Weak Past Tense in Dutch and Low German (PDF) (Thesis). Nijmegan University.
- Rowley, Anthony (1983). "Das Präteritum in den heutigen deutschen Dialekten". Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik. 50 (2): 161–182. JSTOR 40501826.
- Rübekeil, Ludwig (2017). "The dialectology of Germanic". Handbook of Comparative and Historical Indo-European Linguistics. Vol. 2. de Gruyter Mouton. pp. 986–1002. doi:10.1515/9783110523874-013.
- Salmons, Joseph (2017). "The Evolution of Germanic". In Klein, Jared; Joseph, Brian; Fritz, Matthias (eds.). Handbook of Comparative and Historical Indo-European Linguistics: An International Handbook. Vol. 2. de Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9783110523874-014.
- Salmons, Joseph (2018). A History of German: What the Past Reveals about Today's Language (2 ed.). Oxford University Press.
- Sanders, Willy (1974). "Deutsch, Niederdeutsch, Niederländisch". Niederdeutsches Wort. 14: 1–22.
- Saltveit, Laurits (1983). "Anlage der Modussysteme in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1220–1232. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-006.
- Sapp, Christopher D. (2009). "Syncope as the Cause of Präteritumschwund: New Data from an Early New High German Corpus". Journal of Germanic Linguistics. 21 (4): 419–450. doi:10.1017/S1470542709990134.
- Schockaert, Tuur; Van de Velde, Freek (2024). "Een multivariate analyse van de diachronie van het ge-prefix bij het voltooid deelwoord van perfectiva simplicia in het Middelnederlands". Nederlandse Taalkunde. 29 (2): 222–252. doi:10.5117/NEDTAA2024.2.003.SCHO.
- Scott, Alan (2014). The Genitive Case in Dutch and German: A Study of Morphosyntactic Change in Codified Languages. Brill. doi:10.1163/9789004183285.
- Seebold, Elmar (2013). "Die Aufgliederung der germanischen Sprachen". NOWELE. 66 (1): 55–77. doi:10.1075/nowele.66.1.04see.
- Seiler, Guido (2009). "Sound change or analogy? Monosyllabic lengthening in German and some of its consequences". Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics. 12: 229–272. doi:10.1007/s10828-009-9031-y.
- Shrier, Martha (1965). "Case Systems in German Dialects". Language. 41 (3): 420–438. JSTOR 411785.
- Simmler, Franz (1983). "Konsonantenschwächung in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1121–1130. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-005.
- Stiles, Patrick V. (2013). "The pan-West Germanic isoglosses and the sub-relationships of West Germanic to other branches". NOWELE. 66: 5–38. doi:10.1075/nowele.66.1.02sti.
- Stiles, Patrick V. (2017). "The Comparative Method, Internal Reconstruction, Areal Norms and the West Germanic Third Person Pronoun". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 77: 410–441. doi:10.1163/18756719-12340083.
- Swanenberg, Jos (2019). "Grenzen aan de streektaal: De taalkundige en de culturele benadering van het Brabants in Nederland". In Veronique De Tier; Anne-Sophie Ghyselen; Ton van de Wijngaard (eds.). De wondere wereld van de streektaalgrenzen. Leiden: Stichting Nederlandse Dialecten. pp. 39–48.
- Szczepaniak, Renata (2014). "Vowel and consonant epentheses in the history of German from the typological perspective of syllable and word languages". In Caro Reina, Javier; Szczepaniak, Renata (eds.). Syllable and Word Languages. de Gruyter. pp. 160–180. doi:10.1515/9783110346992.160.
- Taeldeman, Johan (2013a). "East Flemish: Phonology". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 211–234. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.211.
- Taeldeman, Johan (2013b). "The southwestern dialect area: Phonology". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 150–173. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.150.
- Taubken, Hans (1996). "Zur Lautgeographie des Westfälischen". Die niederdeutschen Mundarten. Münster: Aschendorf. pp. 2–13.
- Tiefenbach, Heinrich (1987). "-chen und -lein. Überlegungen zu Problemen des sprachgeographischen Befundes und seiner sprachhistorischen Deutung". Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik. 54 (1): 2–27. JSTOR 40502254.
- Van Bree, Cor (2007) [1987]. Historische Grammatica van het Nederlands. bdnl.
- Van Bree, Cor (2013). "The spectrum of spatial varieties of Dutch: The historical genesis". In Frans Hinskens; Johan Taeldeman (eds.). Dutch. Language and Space: An International Handbook of Linguistic Variation, Volume 3. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 100–128. doi:10.1515/9783110261332.81.
- Van Bree, Cor (2016). Leerboek voor de historische grammatica van het Nederlands - Deel 1: Gotische grammatica, inleiding, klankleer (2 ed.). Universitet Leiden.
- Van Bree, Cor (2020). Leerboek voor de historische grammatica van het Nederlands - Deel 2: Flexie woordvorming (2 ed.). Universitet Leiden.
- Van de Velde, Freek (2024). "Dutch". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.935.
- van der Hoek, Michel (2010). "Old Franconian and Middle Dutch <gh> and Velar Palatalization". Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur. 132 (1). doi:10.1515/bgsl.2010.002.
- van der Wal, Marijke J.; Quak, Aad (1994). "Old and Middle Continental Germanic". In König, Ekkehard; van der Auwera, Johan (eds.). The Germanic Languages. Routledge. pp. 72–109.
- van Koppen, Marjo (2017). "Complementizer Agreement". The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Syntax (2 ed.). Wiley Blackwell. pp. 1–140. doi:10.1002/9781118358733.wbsyncom061.
- van Loey, Adolphe (1964). Schönfelds Historische Grammatica van het Nederlands (7 ed.). N.V. W. J. Thieme & Cie.
- van Loey, Adolphe (1970). "ALTNIEDERLÄNDISCH UND MITTELNIEDERLÄNDISCH". In Schmitt, Ludwig E. (ed.). Kurzer Grundriß der germanischen Philologie bis 1500. Band 1: Sprachgeschichte. de Gruyter. pp. 253–287. doi:10.1515/9783110822717.253.
- van Loon, Jozef (2003). "DE CHRONOLOGIE VAN DE R-METATHESIS IN HET NEDERLANDS EN AANGRENZENDE GERMAANSE TALEN". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 1: 141–167. doi:10.1163/18756719-90000136.
- Versloot, Arjen (2016). "Die Endungen -os/-as und -a des Nominativ/Akkusativ Plurals der a-Stämme im Altsächsischen". Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik. 76 (4): 464–477. doi:10.1163/18756719-12340052.
- Versloot, Arjen; Adamczyk, Elżbieta (2017). "The Geography and Dialects of Old Saxon: River-basin communication networks and the distributional patterns of North Sea Germanic features in Old Saxon". In Hines, John; IJssennagger-van der Pluijm, Nelleke (eds.). Frisians and their North Sea Neighbours: From the Fifth Century to the Viking Age. Boydell and Brewer. pp. 125–148. doi:10.1515/9781787440630-014.
- Voyles, Joseph B. (1971). "The Problem of West Germanic". Folia Linguistica Historica. 5 (1–2): 117–150. doi:10.1515/flin.1969.5.1-2.117.
- Wagner, Kurt (1926). "Die Geschichte eines Lautwandels. ks < chs > s". Teuthonista. 2 (1): 30–46. JSTOR 40498432.
- Weijnen, A. (1958). Nederlandse dialectkunde (PDF). Assen: Van Gorcum & Comp.
- Weiß, Helmut (2005). "Inflected Complementizers in Continental West Germanic Dialects". Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik. 72 (2): 148–166. JSTOR 40505274.
- Werlen, Iwar (1983). "Velarisierungen (Gutturalisierungen) in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1130–1136. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-005.
- Werlen, Iwar (1985). Gebrauch und Bedeutung der Modalverben in alemannischen Dialekten. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik Beiheft 49. Franz Steiner Verlag.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1970a). Phonetisch-phonologische Untersuchungen zur Vokalentwicklung in den deutschen Dialekten, Band 1: Die Langvokale im Hochdeutschen. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter & Co. doi:10.1515/9783110881240.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1970b). Phonetisch-phonologische Untersuchungen zur Vokalentwicklung in den deutschen Dialekten, Band 2: Die Diphthonge im Hochdeutschen. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter & Co. doi:10.1515/9783110881240.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1983a). "Die Einteilung der deutschen Dialekte". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 807–900. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-003.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1983b). "Deutsche Dialektgebiete außerhalb des deutschen Sprachgebiets". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 900–930. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-003.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1983c). "Rundung und Entrundung, Palatalisierung und Entpalatalisierung, Velarisierung und Entvelarisierung in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1101–1106. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-003.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1983d). "Dehnung und Kürzung in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1088–1101. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-003.
- Wiesinger, Peter (1983e). "Hebung und Senkung in den deutschen Dialekten". In Besch, Werner (ed.). Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter. pp. 1106–1111. doi:10.1515/9783110203332-003.
- Wiesinger, Peter (2017a) [1975]. "Strukturgeographische und strukturhistorische Untersuchungen zur Stellung der bergischen Mundarten zwischen Ripuarisch, Niederfränkisch und Westfälisch". In Wiesinger, Peter; Patocka, Franz (eds.). Strukturelle historische Dialektologie des Deutschen: Strukturhistorische und strukturgeographische Studien zur Vokalentwicklung deutscher Dialekte. Hildesheim: Georg Olms Verlag. pp. 341–437.
- Wiesinger, Peter (2017b) [1980]. "Die Stellung der Dialekte Hessens im Mitteldeutschen". In Wiesinger, Peter; Patocka, Franz (eds.). Strukturelle historische Dialektologie des Deutschen: Strukturhistorische und strukturgeographische Studien zur Vokalentwicklung deutscher Dialekte. Hildesheim: Georg Olms Verlag. pp. 159–262.
- Zhirmunski, Viktor M. (2010). Naidich, Larissa (ed.). Deutsche Mundartkunde. Peter Lang.