Jump to content

User:Emilyrogers31

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Moral attribution is the process that shapes how we assign responsibility, blame and praise, driving the way we interact with others and defining the foundations of justice and fairness in society (Haidt, 2001). Our view of moral and legal responsibility is intrinsically tied to this process, enabling individuals to engage socially, seek justice, and function in society. Haidt (2001) found that the interaction of intention, outcome, and context in natural moral judgments may significantly impact how individuals assess moral responsibility, influence perceptions of blame or praise, and shape broader societal norms regarding fairness and justice. Moral attribution significantly impacts society by influencing personal relationships, legal decision-making, and wider social factors that contribute to how individuals view moral responsibility and ethical behaviour. Heider (1958) suggested that moral attributes are in need of further research in order to understand the complexity of moral attribution and the role it plays in shaping societal norms and evaluating ethics.


Theoretical Foundations of Moral Attribution


Attribution Theory

Attribution theory is the foundation for understanding moral attribution. It was developed by Heider (1958) and later expanded by Jones and Davis (1965). In moral attribution, this theory suggests that people infer the causes of actions, determining whether they stem from internal factors, such as personal traits or motives, or external factors, like situational constraints or pressures. Correspondent Inference Theory suggests that we attribute moral responsibility based on our judgment of an individual's intentions and freedom to act and, therefore, is crucial in shaping the understanding of moral attribution (Jones & Davis, 1965).

Causal Responsibility and Control

One of the most influential factors in moral attribution is the degree of control an individual has over an action. According to Alicke's (2000) Culpable Control Model, people are more likely to hold others morally responsible when they perceive the actor had control over the outcome and acted intentionally. Volitional Outcome Control, the degree to which an individual can foresee and control the consequences of their actions, is a key aspect of moral attribution. This notion of control is aligned with the concept of free will. For example, if a person is coerced into committing an act due to external pressures, observers may be less likely to consider them morally liable. This, again, is consistent with principles in the attribution theory, where Kelley (1972) explains how external factors reduce moral responsibility.

The Role of Freedom and Determinism

The interplay between freedom and determinism is a key philosphical debate in moral attribution. Libertarians argue that free will is essential for moral responsibility. On the other hand, hard determinists argue that human behaviour is entirely shaped by external factors, implying that individuals cannot be fully morally accountable for their actions (Price, 2023). Frankfurt (1988) introduced the concept of identification with the action: individuals are morally responsible for actions that reflect their values, regardless of external constraints. Frankfurt (1988) believed that the degree to which an individual aligns with the motivations behind their actions determines responsibility rather than whether the action was free from external influence.


Factors Influencing Moral Attribution


Moral Generalisation

Moral attribution tends to generalise moral judgments across different behaviours. People who commit one immoral act, for example, having an affair, are typically perceived as more likely to commit other immoral acts, such as lying or stealing (Birnbaum, 1972). This relates to the Halo Effect, where a negative moral judgement of one behaviour leads to a generalised negative judgement of an individual's overall character (Asch, 1946). These generalisations imply that good or bad behaviour is presumed to reflect an individual's broader moral character (Birnbaum, 1972).

External Constraints and Coercion

External factors, such as coercion and situational pressures, are significant in attributing moral responsibility. Kelley’s Discounting Principle (1972) suggests that individuals are likely to reduce their attribution of personal responsibility when significant external constraints are involved. For example, a soldier committing a war crime under orders might be seen as less morally responsible than someone committing the same crime voluntarily. However, Jones's (1990) and Ross's (1977) research suggests that external constraints are not always fully discounted. In their studies, participants attributed more responsibility to individuals performing constrained actions than the discounting principle would predict, suggesting that individuals may over-attribute personal responsibility regardless of external pressures (Ross, 1977; Jones, 1990).

Identification and Moral Responsibility

Another critical component of moral attribution is Identification, the extent to which an individual desires or endorses the behaviour they have committed, is another key factor in moral responsibility. According to Frankfurt's (1988) theory, moral responsibility is tied to an individual's identification with their actions and whether they fully accept or embrace the motives and consequences of their behaviour. For example, an individual forced into committing a crime might be held morally responsible if they identify with the motivation behind the act, such as financial gain or a desire to please others. Conversely, someone coerced into an act but refuses to identify with the underlying motivation may appear less morally responsible (Frankfurt, 1988).

Attributional and Outcome Responsibility

Another important dimension of moral attribution is Intentionality. An individual may be blamed for an immoral action by having planned, desired, or foresaw the consequences of their actions. Moral consequentialism focuses on the outcomes of actions by holding individuals responsible based on the result, regardless of intent. Alternatively, Deontological Ethics focuses on the morality of the act itself and less on the consequences. Schlenker et al. (1994) argue that individuals perceive responsibility depending on whether the action aligns with an individual's values.


Moral Attribution in Different Contexts


Moral Attribution in Social Interactions

Moral attribution plays a significant role in social interactions. We constantly judge friends and strangers based on perceived intentions, motives, and situational constraints. For example, if someone breaks a promise, we might assign blame according to whether we think the person was in control of the situation or was influenced by external pressures. Ross's (1977) study on fundamental attribution error suggests that individuals often overemphasise personal traits and intentions, even when external situational factors might explain behaviour. In interpersonal relationships, people are frequently held accountable for behaviour that reflects personal traits, even if external pressures contribute to their actions.

Moral Attribution in the Legal System

In the legal context, moral attribution is central to determining guilt and punishment. Legal systems rely on moral judgments to decide whether individuals should be criminally responsible for their actions. Mens Rea (guilty mind) in criminal law is a prime example, focusing on whether the individual intended to commit a crime. Moral consequentialism influences legal decisions when the outcome of an action determines the severity of punishment, even if the intent behind the act was not malicious. The concept of diminished responsibility is based on the idea that external constraints, for example, coercion and mental illness, can reduce an individual's level of moral responsibility (Robinson, 1996). However, legal judgments are often complex since the degree of constraint and its impact on moral responsibility is not easily determined.

Moral Attributional Dilemmas

Ethical dilemmas often complicate moral attributions. When individuals decide between harming one person or saving a group, moral principles can conflict. Utilitarianism focuses on the consequences of an action where asdeontological ethics focuses on the act's morality. These differing perspectives result in varied moral attributions, with utilitarian thinkers assigning responsibility based on outcomes and deontologists focusing on the inherent morality of the act (Foot, 1967).


Research Findings


Moral Judgment and Situational Demands

Goldman (1970), Reeder and Spores (1982) found that situational demands do not always affect moral judgments. They discovered that an individual who has committed adultery was often considered to have low morality, independent of situational demands or pressures that may have been present. However, causal attributions were strongly influenced by situational demands, with observers attributing immoral behaviour more to external circumstances, the more highly such factors were perceived as influencing individuals. These findings highlight the complexity of moral attribution and illustrate the importance of considering nuances in moral judgment.

The Influence of Social Desirability

Perceptions of social desirability also influence moral attributions. According to the Correspondent Inference Theory, socially undesirable behaviour (for example, immoral actions) leads to more extreme moral attributions. This also supports the view that an immoral individual is perceived as having weak moral dispositions. However, socially desirable behaviour is more likely to be attributed to internal dispositions resulting in more moderate judgments (Jones & Davis, 1965; Reeder et al., 1982)


Critical Analysis and Future Directions


Limitations of Current Research

While research on moral attribution has contributed valuable insights, there is still room for further research. Most current research relies on controlled and hypothetical scenarios, which may not reflect the complexities of real-world judgments, reducing the validity of findings (Ross & Nisbett, 1991). In addition, most research into moral attribution has been conducted in WEIRD societies (Western, educated, industrialised, rich, and democratic); therefore, findings cannot be generalised to other cultures (Nisbett & Ross, 1980). The role of situational factors and personality traits in moral judgement also requires more investigation (Hamilton, 1980). Recent technological developments, such as rise of social media, have challenged the understanding of moral attribution. Identities can be anonymous or manipulated in virtual environments, therefore, moral judgments may differ from face-to-face interactions. To understand the consequences and how moral attribution functions in these new situations, further research is required.

Future Directions

Understanding moral attribution is essential for understanding how individuals assign moral responsibility across different social contexts. Further exploration of cross-cultural differences in moral attributions, particularly in non-Western societies, would enrich our understanding of how culture influences moral judgments (Jones et al., 1961; Reeder et al., 1977). Further esearch into how generalising moral judgment influences social perceptions and relations is required, especially when an individual is judged based on an isolated action that can not reflect their character (Ross, 1977).

Conclusion

Moral attribution is essential for engaging human social cognition, enabling individuals to navigate complex morals and ascriptions of responsibilities. This area has been informed, although further insight into influential factors attributions would provide a deeper insight. The interactions and interplay between moral and causal attributions, the influence of various situational factors, and the role of cultural and technological environments need further research. In an age of increasing technological and psychological complexity, the debate over how responsibility should be attributed in various contexts is more relevant than ever.


References

Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556–574. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.556 Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41(3), 258–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0055756 Birnbaum, M. H. (1972). The negativity effect in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(2), 191-198. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034094 Fincham, F. D., & Jaspers, M. L. (1980). The effects of situational factors on attribution: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 87(3), 571-604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.3.571 Foot, P. (1967). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. Oxford Review, 5, 5–15. Frankfurt, H. (1988). The importance of what we care about: Philosophical essays. Cambridge University Press. Goldman, A. (1970). A theory of moral judgment. Journal of Philosophy, 67(7), 214-220. https://doi.org/10.2307/2023461 Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814 Hamilton, V. L. (1980). The role of situational context in moral judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(4), 830-838. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.4.830 Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. American Sociological Review, 23(6), 742. https://doi.org/10.2307/2089062 Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 219-266. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60006-4 Jones, E. E., Harris, V. A., & Laird, M. A. (1961). Attribution of responsibility: The effects of role playing and self-perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23(3), 343-352. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042617 Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and attribution theory. New Directions in Attribution Research, 1, 151-179. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice Hall. Price, E. (2023, December 14). What is libertarian free will? - Answer The Bible. Answer the Bible. https://www.answerthebible.com/what-is-libertarian-free-will/ Reeder, G. D., & Spores, J. M. (1982). Moral attribution and generalization: The role of social desirability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(6), 1264-1276. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.6.1264 Reeder, G. D., & Spores, J. M. (1983). The attribution of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(4), 736–745. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.44.4.736 Reeder, G. D., Messick, D. M., & Van Avermaet, P. (1977). Causal attributions and moral judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(5), 438-444. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.5.438 Robinson, P. H. (1996). Discretion and the sense of justice in the criminal law. Oxford University Press. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173-220). Academic Press. Snyder, M., & Jones, E. E. (1974). The influence of personality on person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(3), 292-302. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035780 Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4(1), 25–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071663