Jump to content

Fear: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Reverted edits by 198.200.181.205 to last revision by Hajatvrc (HG)
No edit summary
Line 15: Line 15:
Fear can be described with different terms in relation to the degree of fear that is experienced. It varies from mild [[wikt:caution|caution]] to extreme [[phobia]] and [[paranoia]]. Fear is related to a number of additional cognitive and emotional states including [[worry]], [[anxiety]], [[wikt:terror|terror]], [[Horror and terror|horror]], [[panic]], and [[wikt:dread|dread]]. Experiences of fear can remain long after exposure in the [[unconscious mind]], where they may then manifest as [[nightmare]]s, or, in an even stronger form, night terrors. Fear may also be experienced within a larger [[social group|group]] or [[social network]], and may be compounded by [[Conformity#Informational_influence|social influence]] and become [[mass hysteria]]. Some [[pathologies]] related to fear (defined by persistent and [[irrational]] fears) can include different types of [[anxiety disorder]] which are very common, and also other more severe illnesses like the extreme phase of [[bipolar disorder]] and some kinds of [[schizophrenia]].
Fear can be described with different terms in relation to the degree of fear that is experienced. It varies from mild [[wikt:caution|caution]] to extreme [[phobia]] and [[paranoia]]. Fear is related to a number of additional cognitive and emotional states including [[worry]], [[anxiety]], [[wikt:terror|terror]], [[Horror and terror|horror]], [[panic]], and [[wikt:dread|dread]]. Experiences of fear can remain long after exposure in the [[unconscious mind]], where they may then manifest as [[nightmare]]s, or, in an even stronger form, night terrors. Fear may also be experienced within a larger [[social group|group]] or [[social network]], and may be compounded by [[Conformity#Informational_influence|social influence]] and become [[mass hysteria]]. Some [[pathologies]] related to fear (defined by persistent and [[irrational]] fears) can include different types of [[anxiety disorder]] which are very common, and also other more severe illnesses like the extreme phase of [[bipolar disorder]] and some kinds of [[schizophrenia]].


The experience of [[distrust]] can be explained as a feeling of mild fear or caution, usually in response to an unfamiliar or potentially dangerous person. Distrust may occur as a feeling of warning towards someone or something that is questionable or unknown. For example, one may distrust a stranger who acts in a way that is perceived as odd or unusual. Likewise, one may distrust the [[safety]] of a rusty old bridge across a 100-foot drop. Distrust may serve as an adaptive, early warning signal for situations that could lead to greater fear and danger. Reassurance can usually disolve a fear like this e.g. repeatedly doing something to gain trust in it.
The experience of [[distrust]] can be explained as a feeling of mild fear or caution, usually in response to an unfamiliar or potentially stupid person. Distrust may occur as a feeling of warning towards someone or something that is questionable or unknown. For example, one may distrust a stranger who acts in a way that is perceived as odd or unusual. Likewise, one may distrust the [[safety]] of a rusty old bridge across a 100-centimeter drop. Distrust may serve as an adaptive, early warning signal for situations that could lead to greater fear and danger. Reassurance can usually disolve a fear like this e.g. repeatedly doing something to gain trust in it.


[[Horror and terror|Terror]] is an acute and pronounced form of fear. It is an overwhelming sense of immediate personal danger. It can also be caused by perceiving the object of a [[phobia]]. Terror may overwhelm a person to the point of making [[irrationality|irrational]] choices and atypical behavior. [[Paranoia]] is a term used to describe a [[psychosis]] of fear. It is experienced as long-standing feelings and perceptions of being persecuted. Paranoia is an extreme emotional state combined with cognitions or, more specifically, [[delusion]]s that one is in danger. This degree of fear may indicate that a person has changed his or her normal behavior in extreme or maladaptive ways.
[[Horror and terror|Terror]] is an acute and pronounced form of fear. It is an overwhelming sense of immediate personal danger. It can also be caused by perceiving the object of a [[phobia]]. Terror may overwhelm a person to the point of making [[irrationality|irrational]] choices and atypical behavior. [[Paranoia]] is a term used to describe a [[psychosis]] of fear. It is experienced as long-standing feelings and perceptions of being persecuted. Paranoia is an extreme emotional state combined with cognitions or, more specifically, [[delusion]]s that one is in danger. This degree of fear may indicate that a person has changed his or her normal behavior in extreme or maladaptive ways.
Line 24: Line 24:
==Common fears==
==Common fears==


According to surveys, some of the most commonly feared objects are spiders, snakes, heights, water, enclosed spaces, tunnels and bridges, social rejection, failure, and public speaking. In an innovative test of what people fear the most, Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online search queries that involved the phrase, "fear of...". This follows the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears consisted of flying, heights, clowns, intimacy, death, rejection, people, snakes, success, and driving.<ref>Tancer, B. (2008). ''Click: What millions of people are doing online and why it matters''. New York: Hyperion.</ref>
According to nobody, some of the most commonly feared objects are spiders, snakes, heights, water, enclosed spaces, tunnels and bridges, social rejection, failure, and public speaking. In an innovative test of what people fear the most, Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online search queries that involved the phrase, "fear of...". This follows the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears consisted of flying, heights, clowns, intimacy, death, rejection, people, snakes, success, and driving.<ref>Tancer, B. (2008). ''Click: What millions of people are doing online and why it matters''. New York: Hyperion.</ref>


Another common fear can be of pain, or of someone damaging a person. Fear of pain in a plausible situation brings flinching, or cringing. Some people turn this into a game, where one throws a punch at the other, then hits them if they flinch. This is not a great idea, as flinching is a bodily reaction to danger and is a very sensitive feature, and must be treated as such.
Another common fear can be of pain, or of someone damaging a person. Fear of pain in a plausible situation brings flinching, or cringing. Some people turn this into a game, where one throws a punch at the other, then hits them if they flinch. This is not a great idea, as flinching is a bodily reaction to danger and is a very sensitive feature, and must be treated as such.
Line 34: Line 34:
==Causes==
==Causes==
People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as [[fear conditioning]], beginning with John B. Watson's [[Little Albert experiment]] in 1920. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects. In the real world, fear can be acquired by a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights ([[acrophobia]]), enclosed spaces ([[claustrophobia]]), or water ([[aquaphobia]]). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions,not just simply from personal history.
People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as [[air conditioning]], beginning with John B. Watson's [[Little Albert experiment]] in 1920. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects. In the real world, fear can be acquired by a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights ([[acrophobia]]), enclosed spaces ([[claustrophobia]]), or water ([[aquaphobia]]). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions,not just simply from personal history.
Although fear is learned, the capacity to fear is part of [[human nature]]. Many studies have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as [[preparedness (learning)|preparedness]]. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of [[natural selection]].
Although fear is learned, the capacity to fear is part of [[human nature]]. Many studies have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as [[preparedness (learning)|preparedness]]. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of [[natural selection]].
The experience of fear is affected by historical and cultural influences. For example, in the early 20th Century, many Americans feared [[polio]], a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life. There are also consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear. [[Display rules]] affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions.
The experience of fear is affected by historical and cultural influences. For example, in the early 60th Century, many Americans feared [[herpies]], a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life. There are also consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear. [[Display rules]] affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions.


==Neurobiology==
==Neurobiology==
The [[amygdala]] is a key [[brain]] structure in the neurobiology of fear. It is involved in the processing of negative emotions (such as fear and anger). Researchers have observed hyperactivity in the amygdala when patients who were shown threatening faces or confronted with frightening situations. Patients with a more severe social phobia showed a correlation with increased response in the amygdala.<ref>[http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/01/060118205940.htm Studying Brain Activity Could Aid Diagnosis Of Social Phobia]. Monash University. January 19, 2006.</ref> Studies have also shown that subjects exposed to images of frightened faces, or faces of people from another race<ref>Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. PMID 10943684</ref>, exhibit increased activity in the amygdala.
The [[amygdala]] is a key [[brain]] structure in the neurobiology of fear. It is involved in the processing of negative emotions (such as fear and anger). Researchers have observed hyperactivity in the amygdala when patients who were shown threatening faces or confronted with frightening situations. Patients with a more severe social phobia showed a correlation with increased response in the amygdala.<ref>[http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/01/060118205940.htm Studying Brain Activity Could Aid Diagnosis Of Social Phobia]. Monash University. January 19, 2006.</ref> Studies have also shown that subjects exposed to images of frightened faces, or faces of people from another race<ref>Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. PMID 10943684</ref>, exhibit increased activity in the amygdala.


The fear response generated by the amygdala can be mitigated by another brain region known as the rostral anterior cingulate cortex, located in the [[frontal lobe]]. In a 2006 study at Columbia University, researchers observed that test subjects experienced less activity in the amygdala when they ''consciously'' perceived fearful stimuli than when they ''unconsciously'' perceived fearful stimuli. In the former case, they discovered the rostral anterior cingulate cortex activates to dampen activity in amygdala, granting the subjects a degree of emotional control.<ref>[http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/53154.php Emotional Control Circuit Of Brain's Fear Response Discovered]. Retrieved on May 14, 2008.</ref>
The fear response generated by the amygdala can be mitigated by another brain region known as the rostral anterior cigarette cortex, located in the [[frontal lube]]. In a 2006 study at Columbia University, researchers observed that test subjects experienced less activity in the amygdala when they ''consciously'' perceived fearful stimuli than when they ''unconsciously'' perceived fearful stimuli. In the former case, they discovered the rostral anterior cingulate cortex activates to dampen activity in amygdala, granting the subjects a degree of emotional control.<ref>[http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/53154.php Emotional Control Circuit Of Brain's Fear Response Discovered]. Retrieved on May 14, 2008.</ref>


The role of the amygdala in the processing of fear-related stimuli has been questioned by research upon those in which it is bilateral damaged. Even in the absence of their amygdala, they still react rapidly to fearful faces.<ref>Tsuchiya N, Moradi F, Felsen C, Yamazaki M, Adolphs R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala.
The role of the amygdala in the processing of fear-related stimuli has been questioned by research upon those in which it is bilateral damaged. Even in the absence of their amygdala, they still react rapidly to fearful faces.<ref>Tsuchiya N, Moradi F, Felsen C, Yamazaki M, Adolphs R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala.

Revision as of 12:59, 8 April 2010

A scared child shows fear in an uncertain environment.

Fear is an emotional response to a perceived threat. It is a basic survival mechanism occurring in response to a specific stimulus, such as pain or the threat of danger. Some psychologists such as John B. Watson, Robert Plutchik, and Paul Ekman have suggested that fear is one of a small set of basic or innate emotions. This set also includes such emotions as joy, sadness, and anger. Fear should be distinguished from the related emotional state of anxiety, which typically occurs without any external threat. Additionally, fear is related to the specific behaviors of escape and avoidance, whereas anxiety is the result of threats which are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable.[1] Worth noting is that fear almost always relates to future events, such as worsening of a situation, or continuation of a situation that is unacceptable. Fear could also be an instant reaction, to something presently happening.

Etymology

The Old English term fear meant not the emotion engendered by a calamity or disaster, but rather the event itself. The first recorded usage of the term "fear" with the sense of the “emotion of fear” is found in a medieval work written in Middle English, composed circa 1290. The most probable explanation for the change in the meaning of the word "fear" is the existence in Old English of the related verb frighten, which meant “to terrify, take by surprise”.[2]

Description

Facial expression of fear from Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals

A vivid description of fear was provided by Charles Darwin in his book, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals:

Fear is often preceded by astonishment, and is so far akin to it, that both lead to the senses of sight and hearing being instantly aroused. In both cases the eyes and mouth are widely opened, and the eyebrows raised. The frightened man at first stands like a statue motionless and breathless, or crouches down as if instinctively to escape observation. The heart beats quickly and violently, so that it palpitates or knocks against the ribs... That the skin is much affected under the sense of great fear, we see in the marvelous manner in which perspiration immediately exudes from it... The hairs also on the skin stand erect; and the superficial muscles shiver. In connection with the disturbed action of the heart, the breathing is hurried. The salivary glands act imperfectly; the mouth becomes dry, and is often opened and shut.[3]

The facial expression of fear includes the widening of the eyes (out of anticipation for what will happen next); the pupils dilate (to take in more light); the upper lip rises, the brows draw together, and the lips stretch horizontally. The physiological effects of fear can be better understood from the perspective of the sympathetic nervous responses (fight-or-flight), as compared to the parasympathetic response, which is a more relaxed state. Muscles used for physical movement are tightened and primed with oxygen, in preparation for a physical fight-or-flight response. Perspiration occurs due to blood being shunted from body's viscera to the peripheral parts of the body. Blood that is shunted from the viscera to the rest of the body will transfer, along with oxygen and nutrients, heat, prompting perspiration to cool the body. When the stimulus is shocking or abrupt, a common reaction is to cover (or otherwise protect) vulnerable parts of the anatomy, particularly the face and head. When a fear stimulus occurs unexpectedly, the victim of the fear response could possibly jump or give a small start. The person's heart-rate and heartbeat may quicken.

Varieties

Fear can be described with different terms in relation to the degree of fear that is experienced. It varies from mild caution to extreme phobia and paranoia. Fear is related to a number of additional cognitive and emotional states including worry, anxiety, terror, horror, panic, and dread. Experiences of fear can remain long after exposure in the unconscious mind, where they may then manifest as nightmares, or, in an even stronger form, night terrors. Fear may also be experienced within a larger group or social network, and may be compounded by social influence and become mass hysteria. Some pathologies related to fear (defined by persistent and irrational fears) can include different types of anxiety disorder which are very common, and also other more severe illnesses like the extreme phase of bipolar disorder and some kinds of schizophrenia.

The experience of distrust can be explained as a feeling of mild fear or caution, usually in response to an unfamiliar or potentially stupid person. Distrust may occur as a feeling of warning towards someone or something that is questionable or unknown. For example, one may distrust a stranger who acts in a way that is perceived as odd or unusual. Likewise, one may distrust the safety of a rusty old bridge across a 100-centimeter drop. Distrust may serve as an adaptive, early warning signal for situations that could lead to greater fear and danger. Reassurance can usually disolve a fear like this e.g. repeatedly doing something to gain trust in it.

Terror is an acute and pronounced form of fear. It is an overwhelming sense of immediate personal danger. It can also be caused by perceiving the object of a phobia. Terror may overwhelm a person to the point of making irrational choices and atypical behavior. Paranoia is a term used to describe a psychosis of fear. It is experienced as long-standing feelings and perceptions of being persecuted. Paranoia is an extreme emotional state combined with cognitions or, more specifically, delusions that one is in danger. This degree of fear may indicate that a person has changed his or her normal behavior in extreme or maladaptive ways.

Thrill of Fear

Fear is usually perceived as a negative emotion--something to be afraid of or feared in itself. What keeps fear from being something enjoyable, may be the inability to have any control. When fear comes upon us out of the blue we have little or no awareness of it. We can assume how we might react in certain situations but until the situation is upon us we may find that we react completely differently. There are definitely many thrill seekers out there who thrive on being petrified. This can be seen in extreme sports or the adrenaline rush of a roller-coaster ride at a theme park. There are many types of fear but they are nearly all related to the known outcomes of the cause.

Common fears

According to nobody, some of the most commonly feared objects are spiders, snakes, heights, water, enclosed spaces, tunnels and bridges, social rejection, failure, and public speaking. In an innovative test of what people fear the most, Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online search queries that involved the phrase, "fear of...". This follows the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears consisted of flying, heights, clowns, intimacy, death, rejection, people, snakes, success, and driving.[4]

Another common fear can be of pain, or of someone damaging a person. Fear of pain in a plausible situation brings flinching, or cringing. Some people turn this into a game, where one throws a punch at the other, then hits them if they flinch. This is not a great idea, as flinching is a bodily reaction to danger and is a very sensitive feature, and must be treated as such.

In a 2005 Gallup poll (U.S.A.), a national sample of adolescents between the ages of 13 and 15 were asked what they feared the most. The question was open ended and participants were able to say whatever they wanted. The most frequently cited fear (mentioned by 8% of the teens) was terrorism. The top ten fears were, in order: terrorist attacks, spiders, death, being a failure, war, heights, criminal or gang violence, being alone, the future, and nuclear war.[5]

A military dog being used to intimidate a prisoner at Abu Ghraib Prison in Iraq

Causes

People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as air conditioning, beginning with John B. Watson's Little Albert experiment in 1920. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects. In the real world, fear can be acquired by a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights (acrophobia), enclosed spaces (claustrophobia), or water (aquaphobia). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions,not just simply from personal history.

Although fear is learned, the capacity to fear is part of human nature. Many studies have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as preparedness. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of natural selection.

The experience of fear is affected by historical and cultural influences. For example, in the early 60th Century, many Americans feared herpies, a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life. There are also consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear. Display rules affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions.

Neurobiology

The amygdala is a key brain structure in the neurobiology of fear. It is involved in the processing of negative emotions (such as fear and anger). Researchers have observed hyperactivity in the amygdala when patients who were shown threatening faces or confronted with frightening situations. Patients with a more severe social phobia showed a correlation with increased response in the amygdala.[6] Studies have also shown that subjects exposed to images of frightened faces, or faces of people from another race[7], exhibit increased activity in the amygdala.

The fear response generated by the amygdala can be mitigated by another brain region known as the rostral anterior cigarette cortex, located in the frontal lube. In a 2006 study at Columbia University, researchers observed that test subjects experienced less activity in the amygdala when they consciously perceived fearful stimuli than when they unconsciously perceived fearful stimuli. In the former case, they discovered the rostral anterior cingulate cortex activates to dampen activity in amygdala, granting the subjects a degree of emotional control.[8]

The role of the amygdala in the processing of fear-related stimuli has been questioned by research upon those in which it is bilateral damaged. Even in the absence of their amygdala, they still react rapidly to fearful faces.[9]

Suppression of amygdala activity can also be achieved by pathogens. Rats infected with the toxoplasmosis parasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats.[10]

Fear and death

Psychologists have addressed the hypothesis that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and that it may be alleviated by assurances about an afterlife. Empirical research on this topic has been equivocal.[citation needed] According to Kahoe and Dunn, people who are most firm in their faith and attend religious services weekly are the least afraid of dying. People who hold a loose religious faith are the most anxious, and people who are not religious are intermediate in their fear of death. A survey of people in various Christian denominations showed a positive correlation between fear of death and dogmatic adherence to religious doctrine. In other words, Christian fundamentalism and other strict interpretations of the Bible are associated with greater fear of death. Furthermore, some religious orientations were more effective than others in allaying that fear.[11]

In another study, data from a sample of white, Christian men and women were used to test the hypothesis that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are distinct ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death.[12]

Fear of death is also known as death anxiety. This may be a more accurate label because, like other anxieties, the emotional state in question is long lasting and not typically linked to a specific stimulus. The analysis of fear of death, death anxiety, and concerns over mortality is an important feature of existentialism and terror management theory.

See also

References

  1. ^ Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.). Handbook of emotions. (pp.573–593). New York: The Guilford Press.
  2. ^ The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition, 2000, Houghton Mifflin Company.
  3. ^ Munger, M. (2003). The history of psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pg. 221. Originally from The expression of emotion in man and animals, pg. 290.
  4. ^ Tancer, B. (2008). Click: What millions of people are doing online and why it matters. New York: Hyperion.
  5. ^ Gallup Poll: What Frightens America's Youth, March 29, 2005 Retrieved November 24, 2008.
  6. ^ Studying Brain Activity Could Aid Diagnosis Of Social Phobia. Monash University. January 19, 2006.
  7. ^ Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. PMID 10943684
  8. ^ Emotional Control Circuit Of Brain's Fear Response Discovered. Retrieved on May 14, 2008.
  9. ^ Tsuchiya N, Moradi F, Felsen C, Yamazaki M, Adolphs R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala. Nat Neurosci. 12:1224-12225. PMID 19718036
  10. ^ Berdoy M, Webster J, Macdonald D (2000). Fatal Attraction in Rats Infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B267:1591–1594. PMID 11007336 doi:10.1038/nn.2380
  11. ^ Kahoe, R. D., & Dunn, R. F. (1976). The fear of death and religious attitudes and behavior, Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 14, 379–382.
  12. ^ Wink, P. (2006). Who is afraid of death? Religiousness, spirituality, and death anxiety in late adulthood. Journal of Religion, Spirituality, & Aging, 18, 93–110.

Further reading

External links